File size: 46,019 Bytes
dae990d |
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846 847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863 864 865 866 867 868 869 870 871 872 873 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 883 884 885 886 887 888 889 890 891 892 893 894 895 896 897 898 899 900 901 902 903 904 905 906 907 908 909 910 911 912 913 914 915 916 917 918 919 920 921 922 923 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 933 934 935 936 937 938 939 940 941 942 943 944 945 946 947 948 949 950 951 952 953 954 955 956 957 958 959 960 961 962 963 964 965 966 967 968 969 970 971 972 973 974 975 976 977 978 979 980 981 982 983 984 985 986 987 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 995 996 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 1003 1004 1005 1006 1007 1008 1009 1010 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1020 1021 1022 1023 1024 1025 1026 1027 1028 1029 1030 1031 1032 1033 1034 1035 1036 1037 1038 1039 1040 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047 1048 1049 1050 1051 1052 1053 1054 1055 1056 1057 1058 1059 1060 1061 1062 1063 1064 1065 1066 1067 1068 1069 1070 1071 1072 1073 1074 1075 1076 1077 1078 1079 1080 1081 1082 1083 1084 1085 1086 1087 1088 1089 1090 1091 1092 1093 1094 1095 1096 1097 1098 1099 1100 1101 1102 1103 1104 1105 1106 1107 1108 1109 1110 1111 1112 1113 1114 1115 1116 1117 1118 1119 1120 1121 1122 1123 1124 1125 1126 1127 1128 1129 1130 1131 1132 1133 1134 1135 1136 1137 1138 1139 1140 1141 1142 1143 1144 1145 1146 1147 1148 1149 1150 1151 1152 1153 1154 1155 1156 1157 1158 1159 1160 1161 1162 1163 1164 1165 1166 1167 1168 1169 1170 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1180 1181 1182 1183 1184 1185 1186 1187 1188 1189 1190 1191 1192 1193 1194 1195 1196 1197 1198 1199 1200 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1210 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1220 1221 1222 1223 1224 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1230 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 1240 1241 1242 1243 1244 1245 1246 1247 1248 1249 1250 1251 1252 1253 1254 1255 1256 1257 1258 1259 1260 1261 1262 1263 1264 1265 1266 1267 1268 1269 1270 1271 1272 1273 1274 1275 1276 1277 1278 1279 1280 1281 1282 1283 1284 1285 1286 1287 1288 1289 1290 1291 1292 1293 1294 1295 1296 1297 1298 1299 1300 1301 1302 1303 1304 1305 1306 1307 1308 1309 1310 1311 1312 1313 1314 1315 1316 1317 1318 1319 1320 1321 1322 1323 1324 1325 1326 1327 1328 1329 1330 1331 1332 1333 1334 1335 1336 1337 1338 1339 1340 1341 1342 1343 1344 1345 1346 1347 1348 1349 1350 1351 1352 1353 1354 1355 1356 1357 1358 1359 1360 1361 1362 1363 1364 1365 1366 1367 1368 1369 1370 1371 1372 1373 1374 1375 1376 1377 1378 1379 1380 1381 1382 1383 1384 1385 1386 1387 1388 1389 1390 1391 1392 1393 1394 1395 1396 1397 1398 1399 1400 1401 1402 1403 1404 1405 1406 1407 1408 1409 1410 1411 1412 1413 1414 1415 1416 1417 1418 1419 1420 1421 1422 1423 1424 1425 1426 1427 1428 1429 1430 1431 1432 1433 1434 1435 1436 1437 1438 1439 1440 1441 1442 1443 1444 1445 1446 1447 1448 1449 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 1485 1486 1487 1488 1489 1490 1491 1492 1493 1494 1495 1496 1497 1498 1499 1500 1501 1502 1503 1504 1505 1506 1507 1508 1509 1510 1511 1512 1513 1514 1515 1516 1517 1518 1519 1520 1521 1522 1523 1524 1525 1526 1527 1528 1529 1530 1531 1532 1533 1534 1535 1536 1537 1538 1539 1540 1541 1542 1543 1544 1545 1546 1547 1548 1549 1550 1551 1552 1553 1554 1555 1556 1557 1558 1559 1560 1561 1562 1563 1564 1565 1566 1567 1568 1569 1570 1571 1572 1573 1574 1575 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 1581 1582 1583 1584 1585 1586 1587 1588 1589 1590 1591 1592 1593 1594 1595 1596 1597 1598 1599 1600 1601 1602 1603 1604 1605 1606 1607 1608 1609 1610 1611 1612 1613 1614 1615 1616 1617 1618 1619 1620 1621 1622 1623 1624 1625 1626 1627 1628 1629 1630 1631 1632 1633 1634 1635 1636 1637 1638 1639 1640 1641 1642 1643 1644 1645 1646 1647 1648 1649 1650 1651 1652 1653 1654 1655 1656 1657 1658 1659 1660 1661 1662 1663 1664 1665 1666 1667 1668 1669 1670 1671 1672 1673 1674 1675 1676 1677 1678 1679 1680 1681 1682 1683 1684 1685 1686 1687 1688 1689 1690 1691 1692 1693 1694 1695 1696 1697 1698 1699 1700 1701 1702 1703 1704 1705 1706 1707 1708 1709 1710 1711 1712 1713 1714 1715 1716 1717 1718 1719 1720 1721 1722 1723 1724 1725 1726 1727 1728 1729 1730 1731 1732 1733 1734 1735 1736 1737 1738 1739 1740 1741 1742 1743 1744 1745 1746 1747 1748 1749 1750 1751 1752 1753 1754 1755 1756 1757 1758 1759 1760 1761 1762 1763 1764 1765 1766 1767 1768 1769 1770 1771 1772 1773 1774 1775 1776 1777 1778 1779 1780 1781 1782 1783 1784 1785 1786 1787 1788 1789 1790 1791 1792 1793 1794 1795 1796 1797 1798 1799 1800 1801 1802 1803 1804 1805 1806 1807 1808 1809 1810 1811 1812 1813 1814 1815 1816 1817 1818 1819 1820 1821 1822 1823 1824 1825 1826 1827 1828 1829 1830 1831 1832 1833 1834 1835 1836 1837 1838 1839 1840 1841 1842 1843 1844 1845 1846 1847 1848 1849 1850 1851 1852 1853 1854 1855 1856 1857 1858 1859 1860 1861 1862 1863 1864 1865 1866 1867 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874 1875 1876 1877 1878 1879 1880 1881 1882 1883 1884 1885 1886 1887 1888 1889 1890 1891 1892 1893 1894 1895 1896 1897 1898 1899 1900 1901 1902 1903 1904 1905 1906 1907 1908 1909 1910 1911 1912 1913 1914 1915 1916 1917 1918 1919 1920 1921 1922 1923 1924 1925 1926 1927 1928 1929 1930 1931 1932 1933 1934 1935 1936 1937 1938 1939 1940 1941 1942 1943 1944 1945 1946 1947 1948 1949 1950 1951 1952 1953 1954 1955 1956 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 1976 1977 1978 1979 1980 1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015 2016 2017 2018 2019 2020 2021 2022 2023 2024 2025 2026 2027 2028 2029 2030 2031 2032 2033 2034 2035 2036 2037 2038 2039 2040 2041 2042 2043 2044 2045 2046 2047 2048 2049 2050 2051 2052 2053 2054 2055 2056 2057 2058 2059 2060 2061 2062 2063 2064 2065 2066 2067 2068 2069 2070 2071 2072 2073 2074 2075 2076 2077 2078 2079 2080 2081 2082 2083 2084 2085 2086 2087 2088 2089 2090 2091 2092 2093 2094 2095 2096 2097 2098 2099 2100 2101 2102 2103 2104 2105 2106 2107 2108 2109 2110 2111 2112 2113 2114 2115 2116 2117 2118 2119 2120 2121 2122 2123 2124 2125 2126 2127 2128 2129 2130 2131 2132 2133 2134 2135 2136 2137 2138 2139 2140 2141 2142 2143 2144 2145 2146 2147 2148 2149 2150 2151 2152 2153 2154 2155 2156 2157 2158 2159 2160 2161 2162 2163 2164 2165 2166 2167 2168 2169 2170 2171 2172 2173 2174 2175 2176 2177 2178 2179 2180 2181 2182 2183 2184 2185 2186 2187 2188 2189 2190 2191 2192 2193 2194 2195 2196 2197 2198 2199 2200 2201 2202 2203 2204 2205 2206 2207 2208 2209 2210 2211 2212 2213 2214 2215 |
So that's best thing. Dr. Richard Francis. That's the they to take the word. What is. Hello. Welcome back. If you are. My name's Sam Solomon. It's my pleasure also to introduce to you today Professor Elliott Carton. He'll be taking a lot of this lecture. So in the last two weeks, we've discussed the different components of brains and brain function. The purpose of this week in general is to help you understand a little bit better to survey the variety of techniques that are available now to study brains and behaviour. To set us up properly for the subsequent weeks, which were on specific terms. So the purpose of today is not to go into in-depth any particular technique. We just like to help you understand the panoply of techniques that are available with the strengths and the limitations of some of those techniques are and why one might choose to employ them in particular circumstances to understand the relationship between brains and behaviour. This is now quite an old slide, or at least most of it is. I adapted it slightly on the x axis is the timescale of which one might like to make a measurement ranging from milliseconds through hours, days and even lifetimes. On the y axis. It's a spatial scale of which one might like to make that measurement from the scale of a single snaps of a single nerve. So through two o columns of those whole brain areas and perhaps even the whole brain, each of the little things in the box are different types of techniques, most of which will encounter some time today. These include things that are, for example, called patch clamp electrophysiology. This is old school electrophysiology, incredibly powerful technique. You put in a glass electrode, so its tip is only 1000 or maybe 5000 millimetre wide. You put that glass tip up against the membrane of a single nerve cell in a slice usually, but sometimes in a whole animal. You suck that little membrane onto the end of the pipette and then you break the seal from the glass into the inside of the cell, what's called a giga arm seal. And that allows you to measure precisely the internal electrical life of that. So that's basically the smaller scale measurement techniques tend to get through on the top. Right, as we'll be discussing soon, is a kind of knowledge you can gain from studying humans or animals with lesions, maybe to large parts of the brain or to even small parts of the brain, often formed by strokes or other kinds of accidents. And in between. These are a range of different techniques that we'll be encountering today, including electrophysiology, calcium imaging, optical imaging that Magneto and Safflower Graham or the electroencephalogram MEG or EEG, but also techniques that are not necessarily aimed at revealing the functional activity of the brain, but the kind of connections between different brain areas. If we were to have this lecture back when the slide was first made. I think the slide was first made back in 1994 because there had been some profusion of techniques, including MRI, into the field. These techniques have now even grown more stronger and more varied in their ability to answer these kinds of questions. Hopefully we'll give you a flavour of that today, so I'll hand off now to the radio to take you through the first component, which is largely to do with how we try and measure brain function in human beings. Hello, everybody. Can you hear me? Yeah. Hi. It's really nice to be here. I'm Andrea. I'm one. This is a professor of experimental psychology, and I mostly do research on human cells quite nicely to telling you about some of the techniques that we use. And we wanted to start a little bit with. Well, the first thing that was available for us to study the human brain. Right? And that was sort of lesions in patients. And that's something that's very much like we call neuropsychology, although neuropsychology implies all those things as well. And you're going to be seeing this a lot through your modules. And so I'm not going to go into a lot of detail about it, but it is fair to say that sort of the groundbreaking studies of Paul Broca were the ones that sort of get this field going in many ways. So Rocco had a patient that had some language production issues, and when that patient, that patient died and then they examined the brain, they saw that they had a lesion in the left hemisphere, in the frontal charges, in the frontal cortex. And they could also see that that happened again with a couple of patients. So that was the first real sort of link between a function behaviour and sort of something localised to a certain part of the brain. And that was really groundbreaking for for neuroscience and for psychology. And it is the technique that we still use up to today. We're going to we're going to talk a lot about hair and burn care during the language recognition lectures. And we also are going to talk about a fascia and how we still use neuropsychology to study the brain in humans. So as I said, it's just really good in terms of getting this still going and making sort of relationships between structure and function in the brain. And what we can do now is also say, okay, well, we have these Asian to have this lesion. Let's try to see how they perform different tasks. And again, you begin to see this a lot. A classic example, the studies of our friend Amelia with patients, H.M. and. So yeah, really groundbreaking as well. That went to a lot of studies. We learned a lot about memory from those and that this technique obviously, you know, has advantages which are like and how we can link brain, um, behaviour and potentially identify regions that are necessary and lesions that never sort of sort of constrained to certain regions of the brain if you want. We don't really know what's damage. The damage will be different between patients. So it's a little bit, it's a kind of like what people call sometimes a nature experiment. Well, someone had a listen. Let's try to understand what happened. And I think in terms of disadvantages was that it was that the lesions are not selective and that also, obviously, the brain has this this capacity of plasticity of of change in potentially structure and function to compensate for what's happening environments is what happened in its own physiology. And so there are a lot of cases that actually the brain compensates over time. So we see this a lot in patients and you will say the cases of aphasia were potentially at the beginning after lesion. A patient has like several sort of potential issues with producing or understanding language. Let's say that as time passes, many of them recover. So not all of them. And that's like a real interesting or like a big area of research trying to understand why a patient will not recover. Many of them do. And that has to do with how the brain can compensate for that damage. However, there are lots of things that you can't really do. You can't really study dynamics between brain regions because you know that lesion is already damaged. So, you know, that's the only thing that you know. And and I think I think for me, the biggest one is that, well, deletion is not selective. So you wouldn't know what exactly is damage or to what extent. And therefore, it's very difficult to make sort of inferences between the structure and the function. We can do it by studying sort of large groups of patients. But for example, there is currently study in Queens at UCL where they're trying to look at that Netflix of yeah, so people would have patients are going to be all the brain lesions and therefore sort of language production and perception problems and they're trying to look at what predicts recovery and they really need to recruit thousands of patients to be able to do this properly. So it is, it is quite hard. Okay. So I'm going to talk now about some other techniques that we can use to actually measure brain function and structure in healthy individuals and in a non-invasive way. And the first one I'd like to talk to you about is magnetic resonance imaging. And I'm just going to show you a video that explains this very well from one of the developers of this technique. If you take, let's say, a human being and put him in one of these big, very homogeneous magnetic field magnets, then there's a tendency of that magnetic field to line up the magnetic moments of the nuclei, the spin of the nuclei and the hydrogen in your body, which is in your muscle and in your blood. Now, put on a radio frequency pulse, say 60 megahertz or something like that. Then you can make this magnetisation of your hydrogen nuclei. You can turn it 90 degrees away from the direction of the magnetic field. Your magnetic moment will process. If you have coils around, pick up coils, they it will induce a signal. If I want to see where the signal is coming from in your body, I put on another magnetic field on top of the very homogeneous one that's called a magnetic field gradient. By that I mean it makes a feel stronger in one place and weaker in another place. What you do in order to actually get the image that you want, the fan resolution that you need, that you put on magnetic field gradients of different strengths, a series of pulses. That's why if you get an MRI machine, you're here. Bom, bom, bom. It's all these pulses that we're putting out with different strengths of magnetic field, perhaps even different directions, because we want to get a three dimensional picture of you. You record all of these data and when you finish, you can now use what's called Fourier transform. It's a mathematical technique. You can work back to how strong the signal was in each of these voxels. And so this is how the image is developed. Okay. So that was a very quick intro to. Right. So am I in the wrong one? Okay, so MRI stands for magnetic resonance imaging, and we usually use this really big magnets, scanners to to sort of scan people to conduct this technique. And functional MRI refers to the kind of specific scanning that is used for measuring, you know, something that relates to brain function. And I what I'm going to expand as I'm going to explain both of them. And so how I. It's just plain. Old. Okay. Thank you for your input on my music selection. And so in Ryan ephemera and material I'm going to talk about a little bit later, it really changed the game in terms of what we could do to study the human brain. And in the last 25 years that these techniques have been around, we have gained a lot of understanding of how many functions that we can only study in detail in humans, such as potential language or some complex decision making or, you know, metacognition and so on, how they work and how how, how the brain represents or like produces its functions. And so how does an MRI what I mean to me, it's absolutely amazing that we can look into the people's brain and actually look into their function without even putting anything inside. We can just take pictures of their brains by measuring how hydrogen atoms move, which is why I was trying to say here. So this is what happens. You put the subject a the participant in a big magnets. The magnets and had a really, really strong magnetic field. And to do that you need liquid helium and you need to keep that constant all the time. So the energy cost of doing this is quite high. And that's why MRI is actually a really expensive technique. And when you put a person in this big magnetic field, as it was thought by all the sort of elite hydrogen atoms were aligned in the same direction. Right. That's why you don't use trying to put them all in the same direction. So then when you sort of disturb them and when a radio wave. Right, they will sort of rotate. When you turn that off, they're going to go back. And when they go back, they're going to miss the signal that you can think about sort of like a kind of singing in a certain frequency. You can see these electrons. And so these hydrogen atoms are thinking in a special frequency when they're going back to the alignment. Right. And that's why we're measuring now how how then can we measure different parts of space if they're all aligned in the same way? That's where you introduce these gradients. So the gradient is just changing the magnetic field slightly. So in a way, you can think about these hydrogen atoms sort of kind of moving and what this thing that's going to be sent is going to be at a different frequency. You can think about it as thinking in a different frequency. So for example, if I wanted to know, you know, how this lecture theatre was populated, but I couldn't see it, I was in a different room. I could just put microphones all around with different with different frequencies and then just have a recording here. Right. So the guys at the very top are going to have really low frequencies, and that's going to be a great note here to very high frequencies. So I'm going to go in the other room and she's going to be recording the signals from all those microphones when you're talking or whatever. And by looking at the frequencies of those recordings, I'm going to be able to exactly say who was sitting where. Because that's the link between frequency and space. And that's why MRI dogs and that's why I like to see pictures of your brain, which is amazing. So if there's like a really strong signal at a specific frequency, I know where it's coming from. That part of of their image is going to look quite bright. And if it's a really low frequency, then it's going to look like that. And that's how we construct and it's really nice brain pictures. And so, yeah, pictures like this, for example. Right. So you can see and this is just a transformation of light, really strong powers. These things are like y by looking wide or depending on what y your technique is, depending on the contrast. And this the opposite for things that, you know, they don't have that much signal. Now this is just to get a structural image. This just gives you a picture of the brain, right, That doesn't link at all to brain function. So what is it that allowed us to use functional MRI to look at brain function? And it is the fact that when a certain part of the brain is active, there will be an increase in blood flow to that part of the brain. When there is an increase in blood flow, there's going to be more haemoglobin, haemoglobin coming into that and that has iron. And it's in a sense the iron is ferromagnetic so interferes with that magnetic field. So by measuring how much interference there is with the magnetic field, we can measure brain function. But as you can see, the images and I'm sure you hear one is the structural image to the kind of images that we use for studying the structure of the brain. And this is a functional image. It's just very low resolution because we actually want to see. How it changes with time. So acquiring one of these images it takes for the whole brain, it takes at least 5 minutes, whereas acquiring this one could take 2 to 3 seconds depending on what you're doing. So we kind of compromising some of the spatial definition to have like a better temporal resolution. But we're still measuring blood flow, and blood flow is quite slow. That's why ephemeral is not a good technique for measuring brain function. So and you think it's no good technique for measuring sort of Air Force temporal events is not it doesn't have a great temporal resolution. That being said, last week there was this really exciting paper published where they had an MRI technique and so milliseconds resolution now like very fast. And it was done in animal models and it was thought like in a single slice. So it's not the whole brain, it's just the very first step towards this technique. But I think, you know, everybody in the field is really exciting because it really looks like in a few years, like maybe ten, 20, who knows? We are going to have a technique that allows like a really great temporal resolution potentially to study the human brain as well. And so when you get these images where you do in the functional case and ephemera is that you take many, many, many of them and then you compare them across conditions. So, for example, let's say here I will record like lots of images do one of my goodness conditions of my experiment, which could be it could be a memory experiment, right? So it could be like memorise these items. And then I have a control condition where I just tell people to, you know, look at them, don't memorise them. And then you can look at how like activity changes across the whole brain. Or like in here you can like look specifically into a certain region, right? So this region here is a few voxels, a voxel instead of the minimal units and ephemera you can look at as a three dimensional pixel. And then we can average, for example, the activity through that. So if you look at this, the intensity in these conditions is going to be different. But this is something that you can't really tell, you know, just by looking at. And the difference is very, very small. It's usually around, you know, between 1% to like 4% or 10%. Best case. And and yeah, it's it's really small, but it is very consistent. So you can measure that and you can measure how it changes. And that's what we're doing here. We have like these three measurements here, for example, from that region in red, and then we compare them to the measurements during the second condition and averages and see this a difference in the amount of like bull's signal in that area. And by doing that across the whole grain, we can come up with these statistical maps where we're showing where in the brain there are significant differences between conditions. Okay. And so as I said, FEMA has brought lots of advantages. I think one of the really nice things is that we can study the dynamics of the whole brain. And, you know, that doesn't only apply to humans, but it's also been used in animal models for this specific purpose as well. And after certain spends, we can understand how different parts of the brain interact and how they form functional networks. So we can like sort of do more complex analysis of network dynamics and see how reaches interact under different situations. And you could do things that we couldn't do for like with patients because, you know, patients essentially have some sort of behavioural deficits. So like there's things that they wouldn't be able to do that we could do now because they're healthy participants. And I'm very nicely we can think we can study human brains, right? We can study things that are so specific to humans that would be difficult to study in all that and which models. So, for example, I study deafness and how the brain of deaf individuals changes. And you know, when the congenitally deaf and continues with deafness in many cases results in a delay in language acquisition, which then has a lot of impact on cognitive functions as well. So it's not that animals are not great for answering those types of questions, so great for studying the effects of sensory deprivation or lack of sensory experience, in this case, a hearing and in the development of the brain in plasticity. And so it not so much to study what are the impacts on cognitive processes. And I think the best example of how we gain from ephemera is we can do things like communicating with patients in a coma. And I really recommend you to look at these videos from Agent Owen and his group where they are using ephemera and what we know about from right. They have been able to communicate with and with patients that before we didn't have any insight into their entire health status. And so that has been a really amazing ground breaking discovery. So I'm not going to go into detail here right now of time. And there are tons of limitations, though. And again, I think know, we have some understanding of what the f MRI signal is. So is this mix of like presynaptic activity and like sort of cells firing as well. And like, we don't know which ones it's coming from. So you never really know what type of activity you measure. And you've just seen a difference in overall blood flow in one region to the other. So that is quite limiting. And and again, the spatial resolution is great, you know, like in comparison with other things. But still, like we're looking at a bunch of cells like and it's going to be quite hard to tell what individual cells or small networks of self are actually doing. And and again, it's like this really big magnets. So people have to stay, you know, sort of still they can move around and that just gives limitations as well. I think, as some said, there's been a lot of advances in these techniques and now we have MRI for humans where we can measure layer specific activity. So different layers of the and, you know, the cerebral cortex. But still, you know, that's still a lot of cells and not specific. Now, before we pass into other techniques, I wanted to talk a little bit about this technique called functional, and yet infrared spectroscopy, which works a little bit like ephemera in sense that it measures changes in blood oxygenation. So you have different sources of light that are attached to the skull, and then you have others that you use for detecting them. And again, if there's a change in blood flow because of haemoglobin, that will that will affect how the light is reflected. So you can measure changes as well. Now, the spatial resolution of this technique is way worse than if it were high, right? Because you just only have a few seconds. So why am I talking about it? What would be the advantage of having this technique? Why do you think? Any ideas? Yeah. Yeah. So exciting. So one of the advantages that you don't need is a big piece of equipment and is way more mobile. Yeah. People. But you were always trying to. Are you? So hospital? Yeah, exactly. They don't have to be like in a specific lab as well, although many cases are. That is exactly that is one of the advantages. So you're sacrificing some spatial resolution to actually be able to do things like, you know, study people moving around or like study children that are not going to be staying very still in an MRI scanner. And I think these are the kind of decisions that we as scientists need to make, as we want to learn about something, we're going to discover something about specific behaviour, about the brain. What's what is the best approach for doing this, given the options that I have? So in cases where you want to study brain function in children, for example, or for example here where you want to sort of like study people interacting and moving around the environment, then obviously it's is going to be a better technique that if I'm right. Um, okay, so there are other techniques that are kind of the opposite of around some that they have great temporal resolution but not so good spatial resolution. And those are magnetic photography and electroencephalography and energy and energy. So EEG have been around for a, for ages and that was kind of the main technique that was used to study the human brain in sort of healthy individuals before any CnF MRI arrive other than just behavioural studies, of course. And so how do they what that means? Just yeah, Okay. So if you have a group of neurones, let's say a line like it will be the case in the cortex, they will have like went in different conditions but actually they will have these currents moving in one direction. Right. And that is what generates EEG signal. That's like the actual electrical activity. And at the same time there will be a magnetic field form surround that current that has gone in one direction. And that's why you can detect with energy. And I think where you can see the main difference in this really retro slide that I call it, because it just happens quite clearly is that if you have this current here, like in the case of ETI, it doesn't go like and in sort of predictable way into the skull because there is like, you know, sort of bones and other tissues and liquid and so on, it kind of deteriorates in a way that is quite difficult to understand where it's coming from. So you can still measure it here, but it's hard to say where it's coming from. And that's why EEG has a pretty bad spatial resolution for energy. This is much better because it is somehow more predictable but still difficult to understand where exactly in the brain this these signals are generated from. And there's been a lot of work in trying to do that. And there have been some advantages based like kind of I mean, when you talk to people that try to understand this and try to sort of generate models of where signals are coming from an EEG, and they basically say this is an impossible problem to solve. Like, you know, we can have like a really good like closed solution as possible, but there's always going to be a few different options. So you're going to have to make decisions in terms of how that now vanishes. There's that that because I mentioned electrical activity, basically, you know, this is direct recording of electrical activity, of neurones firing or the magnetic field is generated and the temporal resolution is excellent. So again, you might want to try to understand what experiments you want to do and whether, you know, this technique is the best. One of these techniques is by a technique that MRI, for example. And so I think I just want to show you again, you can see there's a real difference in set up here as well. So energy, again, has like this really big machine, huge magnetic field. There has to be sort of maintain you could helium. So again, quite expensive, but it now has to stay really, really still whereas EEG is quite portable. You know you can have a cap it's way cheaper as well. So trust me, you took my meds, you were like the v0y is the best thing that I could do. And then you have limitations of what your budget is as well. So, you know, that comes into it as well. And so sometimes you might want to use one or the other. And I kind of did this sort of comparison here to show you what are some of those things that you will have to take into account. So they both have excellent temporary solution, but similarly, they both have problematic spatial resolution, although as I say, energy is way better, right? However, it is way more expensive and participants have to stay very, very still. I would say that applies to each year to some extent as well, but potentially less and the sense. US. I'm in this big machine and that sort of how the people haven't thought. So you can't really move around. So you need this special lab as well. Whereas EEG is way more but more mobile and that spatial resolution is was. And I think it's also is good to take into account that the signals come from different places as well. So I'm not going to go into that. But there are differences in where they're generated as well. And and I think what is really nice as well is that right now here at UCL in Queen Square, in collaboration with all the labs in the UK, there are developments to do with some portable e.g. such as have like a few sensors that potentially measure brain activity in a specific part of the brain so that people can be more mobile and potentially have this much better temporal resolution, so much better spatial resolution with this really good temporal resolution as well. And so I think this is the main so summary of things that you can use for measuring brain activity in humans. I would really recommend you to watch all of these videos, professors from our department talk more about these techniques. And I think more once there's this is not an extensive coverage of all the techniques that you can use to study brain behaviour is more just to give you a flavour of why the possibilities are and what are some of the considerations that you have to take into account when choosing one of those techniques? So I'm going to pass to some now. So it's going to be nice. So the structure of this session is a little bit maybe obscure for you. Just to make clear why Bailey is talking about some things that I'm talking about. Other things is a video works on humans, particularly if humans. But I was always it's not quite always exclusively. I grew up as a scientist working on primates, non-human primates. Both macaque monkeys and marmoset monkeys. And I then moved when I moved to UCLA. One of the reasons I moved here is that you is basically the world centre of focus. But for trying to understand the massive small world they introduced in the last week. Writing about half of France €70 million. So the question I would like to pursue is what I want you to think about. Why would I choose to study animals? And if I'm studying animals, including mine, which seems to be so different from. Why that I'm in the experimental psychology department. Not only can I keep ahead of the problem. So I'd like to then explain to you why I find this such a beautiful experimental technique. It's really exploded over the last ten years or so. I'm going to introduce you to some of those techniques that are really appropriate for the last ten years, and we will be going into those in more detail. We've actually. But basically the reason we use animals or the reason we turned out in some kind to understand why we make based from them. By that I mean that we can and. Make small holes in their brain and introduce devices that we can then use to measure cellular function in those apple. We can never be able to cover. Study. Study. So. But why do my people wear what I love about Merhi? Even in the best case scenario, one includes something like $100,000. But we can look at the active. You need to be in the range and find out how that could be. That single macro might relate to cognitive functions. If the animals, the kinds of things that do have probably going to be very. For example, we're in the very early parts of the program. People that critics are similarly enamoured with. That makes me wonder. I mean. And I think we can learn a lot about how we see or hear, or at least the signals that come from that is or is at the centre of the brain. It's less clear how much we can learn about cognitive structure thought, for example, by studying animals. So it is when meant when we're measuring in invasively would not be very limited circumstances that if we have time we'll get back to at the end of this lecture able to make these recordings from humans and he's already introduced electroencephalogram or EEG. That's a scalp based measurement, something that's non-invasive. All the other things here, invasive measures, we might measure the electrocardiogram, this liquid surface dura between the scalp and the brain measuring almost directly neurones line underneath the electrodes. Or we could use a little invasive, like a little piece of silicon. Let it be inserted into the brain first. Slowly. And located a particular point in the brain. These have different spatial scales of measurement. Electrocardiogram from each of the electrodes from the surface of the brain, intermediate between B, g, and something else. So maybe it measures that mm accumulated the brain issue. The local feel and the electric car they find at the end of one of the electrons moving about the activity of about maybe half of their brain tissue. And then if your electrodes, these little wires on fit have been made well enough, you can actually start to see the. Speaking to people there for 2022 that you that in the next slide we call those things spikes. So this might be the signal that you would you would acquire from one of the white actor to. And the top. This is 3 to 3 traces of the same trace. On the top is what we would estimate as the local fuel potential. That's the sum that at the end of the start to activity between the records and the ground, my whole area. And you can see that normally it's a local film that was varying fairly slowly as the red line traces out some of the slow fluctuations. It's traced maybe a second or 3 seconds long. And you can see that it is dominated by these two things that are going. But it would be very gratifying to deal with these fluctuations if we then are able to look at those little rapid events and those are indicated by the red dots here. We will see that they are the extracellular signal of the action that we discussed in the last lecture. How next? Looks like from outside the city mirror. There's not quite. Now because his electrode is still quite large and there's many cells around the end of those action potentials will come from one another. It doesn't really look. Really? Okay. This better here. Okay, I'll do that. Thank you. So the point to hear the difference down here, sir. Thank you for that. Each of each of those made. It is like five or ten nerve cells around the tip of the electrode. Each of them are producing action potentials occasionally. Often you cannot distinguish between the shapes of the action potentials. Just find your own one on your own. Two on your own. Three on your own. For. So we can combine these action potentials and say, well, they're coming from single nerve cells. It's just a few of them. Five or ten of those may be in the local area. And therefore, we would call this activity the multi-unit activity. So there's multiple units. And by the way, you will come across this word unit quite frequently in this course. That just means a nerve cell in the brain recorded in this way. But sometimes if the electorate really is really nice and it's really close to the so some of her neurones in the brain tissue, you see a reproducible waveform. Looks the same every time you identify in the recording and that we would call single unit activity the activity of a single nerve cell, the same nerve cell on each turn, so able to record with this piece of wire or multiple pieces of wire. The activity of these nerve cells in the whole living brain, in animals that are awake, behaving, even moving around all environments. So we can. I think that this technique, which has been around now for several decades, try to recall the activity of nerve cells in the brain. Oops. This video shows you a video from the laboratory of Torsten Wiesel and David Hubel trained maybe 50 years ago. They won the Nobel Prize for these recordings. These videos are hard to find, so I'd like to show it here. You're going to hear you can actually play those phrases I showed you through an audio speaker and you can actually listen to the action potentials that are being fired by neurone. In this case, they're recording from the visual cortex of a cat. And they're able to work out what this neurone is saying by presenting different visual stimuli. And you'll be able to see that during the course of this video. Well, you can hear the. As I continue to continue to find. What they're doing is they're going to the frontier now where it's going to have to be a little crazy for these things to be generated. Reporter But this is a receptive field. Getting into that. And this particular unit is selected for the director most of the time and. Academic Congressional. Important political orientation of the. So that video just shows you you can use this technique to see that these nerve cells in the visual cortex are selective with the orientation, the motion direction of of a visual stimulus. And that's why they won the Nobel Prize. By using this technique, you would not have been able to determine that if you looked at the MRI signal, which is something over hundreds of thousands of nerve cells. You have to look at individual nerve cells to be able to work that out. Oops. So the ultimate goal of all this new activity is to control behaviour. And I said that there were some limited circumstances in which we could make these measurements in humans, including people, for example, who are suffering from epilepsy, where we need to measure from the brain to work out whereabouts the surgical intervention could take place. It also includes from people who in this case are suffering from paraplegia, automatically paralysis. Where the hope is that we recording the activity of ourselves. Cells will be able to help them regain function of a robotic limb. You have to effectively take what their brain, which is still intact, is sending the signals that are sending and use that to control something that we can then help them move around the world. In this particular case, these devices, which are often called utile rays, which we have used in animals, I've never used them in humans. They were developed from being used in humans, have been implanted in little part of the brain for the motor cortex, which is important in generating movements. And we'll get that in about two weeks time. I just wanted to show you this video briefly because it's incredibly evocative, like the Parkinson's video we watched the other week and say to you, when we know how we can interpret the signals and nerve cells, how powerful that can be. In this paper to people with Tetra and as two people who were unable to move their arms or legs in any functional, useful way, were able to control a prosthetic or a robotic arm simply by thinking about the movement of their own paralysed hand. And they did that using the investigational BrainGate neural interface system. So they thought about using their own arm and hand as though they were reaching out themselves with their own limb. And the robotic arm moved much the way their own arm would have moved. One of the longstanding questions not only in neuroscience but in neuro rehabilitation, is whether the cells in the motor cortex and other parts of the brain, whether they continue to function the same way years after that original injury. It is possible for people to use their thoughts to control devices, either a computer or a robotic arm. The way that happens is that we implant a tiny sensor just about the size of a baby aspirin just into the surface of the brain, and that sensor pick up the electrical impulses from a bunch of neurones. And each of those neurones are like radio broadcast towers putting out impulses. And when they get to the outside, the computer translated converts the pattern of pulses into something that is a command. One of our participants was able to do something that when all of a sort for the first time gave us all pause. She reached out with the robotic arm. She thought about the use of her own hand. She picked up that thermos of coffee, brought it close to her, tilted it towards herself, and sipped coffee from a straw. That was the first time in nearly 15 years that she had picked up anything and been able to drink from it solely of her own volition. There was a moment of true joy, true happiness. I mean, it was beyond the fact that it was an accomplishment. I think an important advance in the entire field. So I want to say that because actually that work has, as we'll go into it next week, based on the foundation of recording from these individual nerve cells or small groups of them, in this case, maybe 50 or 100 nerve cells from the motor cortex. And because we know what those individual nerve cells are doing, and because we can record them simultaneously with these kinds of devices, we can infer what the signal is that she was trying to send her now arms at. And now she's now disconnected from because she has paraplegia. And because we were able to look at those individual nerve cells, we can interpret that signal in ways that is able to involve a prosthetic limb. Now, when we when that device came out into the market about 15 years ago, it seemed like a game changer move from one electrode to 100 electrodes. Well, recently and this is work from Nick Steinmetz and colleagues from just across the road here. We've developed new devices that can actually record from thousands of neurones. At the same time. These are sometimes called neuro pixel devices. We use these routinely. Most people, many people use they'll now use these routinely. We can record from multiple brain areas. Sometimes the individual nerve cells, in each case a groups of them within each of these brain areas. And I don't want you to try and understand what's on this line in terms of signals and measuring. But the point is that we can start to interpret how individual nerve cells across the brain are working together to make cognitive decisions in these small animals lives which share some of our brain capacity. The other technique that you'll be exposed to over the next few weeks is called calcium imaging. I won't again, I won't take you through this in any great detail. It relies on the fact that every time an action potentially is produced, I've talked to you about how sodium ions come flexing into the cell. There's also a little thing called calcium line to become flexing in the cell. And by designing particular molecules to sense the presence of those calcium lines and make line signals dependent on how many calcium lines are present, we can actually engage with light the internal activity of so many cells at once. We make injections or use animals of being transgenic engineered to express these little proteins in many cells in the cortex. And we can use funky little microscopes or very large ones, actually very expensive ones, to try and image the activity of those cells in the brain without, in this case, electrodes rather, using light to record the activity of these nerve cells. And this particular technique has a visual advantage with electrical recordings. We can't work out which cell we were recording from. We know it was a single cell, but we don't know which side it was. But with calcium imaging, with imaging the brain, we actually know which cell produced the activity. We can take the brain down the animal after the experiment, having killed the animal and then remove the brain. And then we can process the brain tissue in particular ways that allows us to look at the anatomical structure of that brain circuit so we can take this functional activity is activity recorded in, say, a hundred neurones in a particular part of the brain and relate it to the structural connections between those neurones and between those neurones and the rest of the brain is not to bring together. Finally, how does individual nerve cells work together in a circuit to provide cognitive function? So that's why I'm interested in studying animals, because I think that it's only by looking at the real structure, the size structure of new activity in these circuits that we'll understand the structure of cognition. Now, I think they might disagree and say that animals don't have interesting enough cognitive functions to allow us to make much inference about humans. But I hope that there will be some intersection between those two things where we can actually find some cognitive function in animals that we find interesting as humans, and where we can understand the neural circuits that actually allow that cognitive function. So that's the span of the different techniques that we might come across in this course. The next election will be about how we can apply some of these to, for example, rehabilitation. And then after that, from next week onward, movies are about specific systems, specific pathways for the brain. We better wrap it up there as as was last week, and you lead by this talk as well so we can. Thanks, everyone. During. Hi. Hi. Yeah. Yeah, I know. I worked that out this morning. You think? Yes, I'll do that. Thanks for reminding me. Like mom and. Dad. Oh, fantastic. Love to see that. Yes. Good. All right, well, I'll send those things to you today. Anything else? Not at the moment, I think. Thanks. I know at the amount of money. Tucker Carlson get. At the end of last week, they spoke briefly and say how he's doing. Yeah, I just want to know that we know from. I can't blame the people. Who are involved in the rescue issue. In 1945. Clinical Psychology. I think having different people on the floor. If you if you get. I don't get the story. I didn't. I didn't. Pretty promptly. It's getting better. I found last week because I'm given a lecture and I live next to my two or three years. Pacing was completely wrong. Yeah, I know that. She goes. Is getting inadequate lecture fees? Yes. Down here. Yeah. I knew about luck. Because the natural instinct is to end up that way. Yeah. Last week Hobson did it, and it's a week away from doing it. That's right. There's no reason show will be the best. But. Oh, come into right now. He's like, friend. We think. That there. 41. Former. Better. It's been more than a game. She. Can. But. You. Life. Yeah. So that was the thing that I think everybody. That. Great. And you? I want to point out. Thank. I didn't. So. So. Okay. |