File size: 40,473 Bytes
dae990d |
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846 847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863 864 865 866 867 868 869 870 871 872 873 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 883 884 885 886 887 888 889 890 891 892 893 894 895 896 897 898 899 900 901 902 903 904 905 906 907 908 909 910 911 912 913 914 915 916 917 918 919 920 921 922 923 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 933 934 935 936 937 938 939 940 941 942 943 944 945 946 947 948 949 950 951 952 953 954 955 956 957 958 959 960 961 962 963 964 965 966 967 968 969 970 971 972 973 974 975 976 977 978 979 980 981 982 983 984 985 986 987 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 995 996 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 1003 1004 1005 1006 1007 1008 1009 1010 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1020 1021 1022 1023 1024 1025 1026 1027 1028 1029 1030 1031 1032 1033 1034 1035 1036 1037 1038 1039 1040 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047 1048 1049 1050 1051 1052 1053 1054 1055 1056 1057 1058 1059 1060 1061 1062 1063 1064 1065 1066 1067 1068 1069 1070 1071 1072 1073 1074 1075 1076 1077 1078 1079 1080 1081 1082 1083 1084 1085 1086 1087 1088 1089 1090 1091 1092 1093 1094 1095 1096 1097 1098 1099 1100 1101 1102 1103 1104 1105 1106 1107 1108 1109 1110 1111 1112 1113 1114 1115 1116 1117 1118 1119 1120 1121 1122 1123 1124 1125 1126 1127 1128 1129 1130 1131 1132 1133 1134 1135 1136 1137 1138 1139 1140 1141 1142 1143 1144 1145 1146 1147 1148 1149 1150 1151 1152 1153 1154 1155 1156 1157 1158 1159 1160 1161 1162 1163 1164 1165 1166 1167 1168 1169 1170 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1180 1181 1182 1183 1184 1185 1186 1187 1188 1189 1190 1191 1192 1193 1194 1195 1196 1197 1198 1199 1200 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1210 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1220 1221 1222 1223 1224 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1230 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 1240 1241 1242 1243 1244 1245 1246 1247 1248 1249 1250 1251 1252 1253 1254 1255 1256 1257 1258 1259 1260 1261 1262 1263 1264 1265 1266 1267 1268 1269 1270 1271 1272 1273 1274 1275 1276 1277 1278 1279 1280 1281 1282 1283 1284 1285 1286 1287 1288 1289 1290 1291 1292 1293 1294 1295 1296 1297 1298 1299 1300 1301 1302 1303 1304 1305 1306 1307 1308 1309 1310 1311 1312 1313 1314 1315 1316 1317 1318 1319 1320 1321 1322 1323 1324 1325 1326 1327 1328 1329 1330 1331 1332 1333 1334 1335 1336 1337 1338 1339 1340 1341 1342 1343 1344 1345 1346 1347 1348 1349 1350 1351 1352 1353 1354 1355 1356 1357 1358 1359 1360 1361 1362 1363 1364 1365 1366 1367 1368 1369 1370 1371 1372 1373 1374 1375 1376 1377 1378 1379 1380 1381 1382 1383 1384 1385 1386 1387 1388 1389 1390 1391 1392 1393 1394 1395 1396 1397 1398 1399 1400 1401 1402 1403 1404 1405 1406 1407 1408 1409 1410 1411 1412 1413 1414 1415 1416 1417 1418 1419 1420 1421 1422 1423 1424 1425 1426 1427 1428 1429 1430 1431 1432 1433 1434 1435 1436 1437 1438 1439 1440 1441 1442 1443 1444 1445 1446 1447 1448 1449 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 1485 1486 1487 1488 1489 1490 1491 1492 1493 1494 1495 1496 1497 1498 1499 1500 1501 1502 1503 1504 1505 1506 1507 1508 1509 1510 1511 1512 1513 1514 1515 1516 1517 1518 1519 1520 1521 1522 1523 1524 1525 1526 1527 1528 1529 1530 1531 1532 1533 1534 1535 1536 1537 1538 1539 1540 1541 1542 1543 1544 1545 1546 1547 1548 1549 1550 1551 1552 1553 1554 1555 1556 1557 1558 1559 1560 1561 1562 1563 1564 1565 1566 1567 1568 1569 1570 1571 1572 1573 1574 1575 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 1581 1582 1583 1584 1585 1586 1587 1588 1589 1590 1591 1592 1593 1594 1595 1596 1597 1598 1599 1600 1601 1602 1603 1604 1605 1606 1607 1608 1609 1610 1611 1612 1613 1614 1615 1616 1617 1618 1619 1620 1621 1622 1623 1624 1625 1626 1627 1628 1629 1630 1631 1632 1633 1634 1635 1636 1637 1638 1639 1640 1641 1642 1643 1644 1645 1646 1647 1648 1649 1650 1651 1652 1653 1654 1655 1656 1657 1658 1659 1660 1661 1662 1663 1664 1665 1666 1667 1668 1669 1670 1671 1672 1673 1674 1675 1676 1677 1678 1679 1680 1681 1682 1683 1684 1685 1686 1687 1688 1689 1690 1691 1692 1693 1694 1695 1696 1697 1698 1699 1700 1701 1702 1703 1704 1705 1706 1707 1708 1709 1710 1711 1712 1713 1714 1715 1716 1717 1718 1719 1720 1721 1722 1723 1724 1725 1726 1727 1728 1729 1730 1731 1732 1733 1734 1735 1736 1737 1738 1739 1740 1741 1742 1743 1744 1745 1746 1747 1748 1749 1750 1751 1752 1753 1754 1755 1756 1757 1758 1759 1760 1761 1762 1763 1764 1765 1766 1767 1768 1769 1770 1771 1772 1773 1774 1775 1776 1777 1778 1779 1780 1781 1782 1783 1784 1785 1786 1787 1788 1789 1790 1791 1792 1793 1794 1795 1796 1797 1798 1799 1800 1801 1802 1803 1804 1805 1806 1807 1808 1809 1810 1811 1812 1813 1814 1815 1816 1817 1818 1819 1820 1821 1822 1823 1824 1825 1826 1827 1828 1829 1830 1831 1832 1833 1834 1835 1836 1837 1838 1839 1840 1841 1842 1843 1844 1845 1846 1847 1848 1849 1850 1851 1852 1853 1854 1855 1856 1857 1858 1859 1860 1861 1862 1863 1864 1865 1866 1867 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874 1875 1876 1877 1878 1879 1880 1881 1882 1883 1884 1885 1886 1887 1888 1889 1890 1891 1892 1893 1894 1895 1896 1897 1898 1899 1900 1901 1902 1903 1904 1905 1906 1907 1908 1909 1910 1911 1912 1913 1914 1915 1916 1917 1918 1919 1920 1921 1922 1923 1924 1925 1926 1927 1928 1929 1930 1931 1932 1933 1934 1935 1936 1937 1938 1939 1940 1941 1942 1943 1944 1945 1946 1947 1948 1949 1950 1951 1952 1953 1954 1955 1956 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 1976 1977 1978 1979 1980 1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015 2016 2017 2018 2019 2020 2021 2022 2023 2024 2025 2026 2027 2028 2029 2030 2031 2032 2033 2034 2035 2036 2037 2038 2039 2040 2041 2042 2043 2044 2045 2046 2047 2048 2049 2050 2051 2052 2053 2054 2055 2056 2057 2058 2059 2060 2061 2062 2063 2064 2065 2066 2067 2068 2069 2070 2071 2072 2073 2074 2075 2076 2077 2078 2079 2080 2081 2082 2083 2084 2085 2086 2087 2088 2089 2090 2091 2092 2093 2094 2095 2096 2097 2098 2099 2100 2101 2102 2103 2104 2105 2106 2107 2108 2109 2110 2111 2112 2113 2114 2115 2116 2117 2118 2119 2120 2121 2122 2123 2124 2125 2126 2127 2128 2129 2130 2131 2132 2133 2134 2135 2136 2137 2138 2139 2140 2141 2142 2143 2144 2145 2146 2147 2148 2149 2150 2151 2152 2153 2154 2155 2156 2157 2158 2159 2160 2161 2162 2163 2164 2165 2166 2167 2168 2169 2170 2171 2172 2173 2174 2175 2176 2177 2178 2179 2180 2181 2182 2183 2184 2185 2186 2187 2188 2189 2190 2191 2192 2193 2194 2195 2196 2197 2198 2199 2200 2201 2202 2203 2204 2205 |
Okay, let's let's get started. Everyone. Can everyone hear me? I'm not used to speaking in such a big lecture theatre. Okay. I'm Dan Bender. I'm the associate professor of Behavioural Neuroscience. My lab studies sleep, among other things. And that's what I'm going to talk about today. I do have to leave at a little bit before 11 for sure. So if there are any lingering questions, I'll have my email address over here and you can email me. But my P.A. soon is operating and I can't miss her presentation. So we'll we'll jump right in. So before I talk about sleep, okay, which is a very interesting topic, I want to take something. Go back a little more basic and ask the question, why do animals have a brain? Right. That's that's probably one of the most basic things in neuroscience. Why do we have a brain? And we look through that animal kingdom. We can see that you have sort of the most primitive species, such as sponges. Okay. And these actually don't have any neurones at all. Okay. So the first animals did not have neurones. It happened some point in evolution. Okay. And you see that neurones appear around the time that jellyfish and similar creatures developed in evolution. And then if we get to more complex animals, it's no longer just neurones, but we have ganglia and brain. Okay. So it's clusters of neurones working together to do something that individual neurones can't. Now, this is where it gets interesting that if we look at our closest one of our closest invertebrate invertebrate relatives, the starfish. So the starfish is closer than roundworms and molluscs and insects. It's closer to us than all those other animals. Starfish actually lost the ganglia. They lost the brain. They just have neurones that are connected together in a ring. Okay. Same with sea urchins. Next time you have uni for sushi, you're thinking you're eating one of your closest relatives in the invertebrate kingdom. Now, where it gets very interesting is sea squirts. The sea squirts actually have a brain in the larval stage, but then they lose it when they're an adult. And if we look at sea squirts, it gives us an answer to the question, Why do we have a brain? Okay. So if we look at a larval sea squirt, it looks a bit like a tadpole. It has two little eyes spots. It has a tail. It's not a spine, it's not a vertebrate, but it has a new record, which is, you know, very close to what vertebrates would have. And it has a digestive system. And its main goal and its early life is to find a nice rock, because once it finds a nice rock, it's going to attach that rock for the rest of its life. And that rock is where it gets all its food and acts as a filter filtering the water. So if you place a bad rock, it kind of sucks, right? That's the rest of his life. It's there. So the thing that's so interesting is that when you're attached to a rock, why would you need to see it if a predator was coming? You can't escape from it. You're stuck to the rock. So vision is useless. Motion is useless, right? You're just stuck to the rock. So you're just floating in the water. So what does a sea squirt do? It eats its nervous system. Okay. It doesn't need it anymore. It's useful energy. It digests it. Okay. So here's the thing. If you don't need to move, you don't need to see. You don't need a brain. Okay. So why do animals have a brain? Animals need a brain to convert sensation to action. A jellyfish does that and moves around the water looking for food. A sponge doesn't. It just filters the water. Doesn't need a brain. Okay. Now, this is where it leads to sleep. Every animal thus far that we know of, if. If an animal moves, it sleeps. Okay? Sleep is a really primitive aspect of the brain. How do we measure sleep? Right. You can't measure it in a jellyfish. It seems know when you wake it up, it snores. You have no way of measuring it, you would think. But it turns out that the most primitive forms of sleep there is a lack of motion for a prolonged period of time. Much like us, there is an elevated threshold to react to sensory stimulus. Right. So if I mention your name now, you're. You know, you'll look around, right? You'll hear me. If you were sleeping in class, you might not unless I was loud enough. Right. Don't worry. I won't test that out. The third thing, and this is a bit harder to test, but you can test in a lot of animals. There is a rebound after sleep deprivation. Right. Which. Which makes sense. If I. If I keep you for three days without sleeping, you're probably going to fall asleep really, really fast and sleep for a longer period of time to compensate. Okay. So those there is of course, sleep is a lot more complex. When you talk about mammals. But the most basic definition of sleep are these three things. So now you can ask the question when you move up the food chain in evolution, how to sleep in the animal kingdom. So let's start with the jellyfish. This is the upside-down jellyfish. I believe this was a was an undergraduate project from a bunch of students, and it got published in Current biology. That's the story I heard. So they had a bunch of jellyfish in an A tank and they tracked the activity of the jellyfish. And they found that in the daytime it was moving a lot. And the Night-Time it wasn't moving so much. Now, that does not mean that the animal is sleeping on its own. Right. During this class, some of you might not be moving a lot. It doesn't mean that you're sleeping. Right. So how can you tell the difference? So what they did is they perturb the animal so it couldn't rest. Okay. There would be a gust of water coming up. And every time the animal was sort of, you know, resting, it would have to move again. And what they found is that when they perturbed one night asleep, there was a recovery where there was less activity than you would expect prior to the perturbation. Okay. And even when the animal slept over here, we would consider sleeping. There was even less activity. So it was almost like a like you could say a deeper sleep. Now, this is maybe not the same as being able to record EEG and EMG. You know, all the signals from the brain of humans and monitoring their sleep. But this is the jellyfish, right? And we're already seeing some basic aspects of sleep here. Okay. Remember, they don't have a brain. They just have neurones. That's key. Now, if we go up the food chain to the fruit fly, which is obviously a lot more complicated than the jellyfish and its nervous system, we see that the fruit fly rests more at Night-Time. You know, lack of movement than in the daytime, which is already good because usually sleep doesn't have to be 12 hours a night and you're awake 12 hours during the day. There are lots of different ways sleep can manifest. But you do see that there is a preferred time for the animal to rest. And what they can test here is also the arousal threshold that if you, you know, lightly do an air popper, do something that would preserve the animal. You have to put more force to get the animal to wake up. Okay. So an animal to move. So this is one of the key things for sleep. It's not simply the animals not moving, but it's paying attention. And you have to somehow break through. It's it's an intention to to get through the animal. Okay. Now, once we're in the fruit fly. There is one other thing that's quite cool that you can start doing that this would be very difficult to do with the jellyfish, but you can put electrodes in the brain. You can tell the animal in a ball so it walks around and you can actually record neural activity and combine this with the movement of the animal. So now you have a way to to not just look at sleep behaviourally, but look electrophysiological of what's going on, to see if there are characteristics of neural signals that that tell you, oh, the animal is sleeping because you see this animals awake because you see this. Okay. Now we're going to go to a larger animal now, the lizard and the lizard here. You know, it's it's a vertebrate, first of all. Right. It's a lot closer to us. They definitely sleep. Okay. And in the lizard, you start seeing it. Sleep is not just a homogeneous pattern in the brain. There's actually two stages. Now, we don't know if it's exactly like the two stages of sleep in humans, but it does seem to be one stage where you have synchronous activity in one stage where it's less synchronous. And these seem to oscillate back and forth. So it's quite different from what you see in other mammals. But you already start seeing neural signatures of two different types of sleep. And reptiles. Okay. Now when you get to birds and mammals, now you have a more defined sleep pattern. And this is typically called slow wave sleep and REM sleep, Slow wave sleep. You can also call non-REM sleep. But essentially, when you go to sleep, you fall into deep sleep. And you have a lot of this in your early sleep. And then you have a little bit of REM sleep. And then each cycle of sleep, which is about 90 minutes, you have a little bit more REM and a little bit less slow wave. Okay. And by the end of the night, it's mostly REM sleep. Okay. Show of hands. Do you typically dream in your slow wave sleep or REM sleep? Raise your hand if it's slow. Wave sleep. Raise your hand in his REM sleep. Good. So REM sleep is where you typically dream, but you can dream also and slowly sleep. You typically remember your dreams from REM sleep more. Okay. So when you're in slow wave sleep and you put up electrodes so you can record neural activity, you can see that there's slow oscillations which relate to cortical cortical communication. There's spindles which are high oscillation signals that relate to the thalamus talking to cortex. And there are also sharp wave ripples, which are extremely high frequency oscillations in the hippocampus that help it talk to cortex. In REM sleep, you see completely different brain activity. You see theta activity in the hippocampus and you see tiger waves as well. Okay. So if you just were watching the signals coming from the brain, you could tell if someone is sleeping and whether it's REM sleep or non REM sleep. Okay. And the key thing to remember is REM sleep activity is very similar to awake activity that is not synchronised and sort of like all the neurones. Imagine if the orchestra and there all every instrument is just practising on its own. They don't seem coordinated. Slow wave sleep. The whole orchestra is going boom, boom, boom together. And that's what the slow waves are. Okay, so you have these two different types of sleep. Now we can look at the animal kingdom to see how does this type of sleep? Very right. Do all animals sleep like us or is there a lot of variability? And what you find is that at least with REM sleep, it is really, really different across the animal kingdom. So the platypus is the king of REM sleep. They have 8 hours a night. We only have two. Okay. The dolphin is unfortunately the animal with one of the least amount of REM sleep. They have minutes every night. Okay. So we know REM sleep is really important. The same time. That dolphin is a lot smarter than the platypus. So it's not really clear why all this extra sleep a platypus is getting what it gains from us. Now, if you look at overall sleep times across the animal kingdom, one thing you notice is that two animals that are genetically very similar can have very different amounts of sleep. So a nice example is the owl monkey below over here compared to the human right. We're both primates. We actually both have the same amount of REM sleep per night, about 2 hours. But the owl monkey sleeps 17 hours. We sleep well. We're supposed to sleep 8 hours. It's probably a lot less. In general, you're staying up late at night, you know, with your Twitter accounts and bingeing Netflix, like. Like all of us. So it's not just genes, right? There's something else going on. And it's very likely to be ethology, which means there are specific constraints on animals, behaviour and habitat. So, for instance, if you could sleep all day, right, because you didn't have to worry about food and resources and other things. Yeah, maybe. Maybe over many, many millions of years, you would start sleeping more, right? Another thing that's quite buried in the animal kingdom is unit hemisphere of sleep. So we sleep with both hemispheres. At least most of us do. The dolphins are famous for sleeping with one hemisphere than the other hemisphere. And you can see this as they will sort of circle around when they're sleeping with one side and then they circle the other direction when they're sleeping with the other part of their brain. And they have to do this because. They need to breathe consciously, so they have to keep part of their brain on. Otherwise, they die. Okay. So it turns out that it's not just dolphins that are cool, that there are lots of species that are capable of this, including birds, water, animals, reptiles. It's almost that we are the oddballs of the year. And if you think about it, it's it's quite smart that you always keep part of your brain on is if a predator comes or you need to be able to do something, you would react faster, right? Or your brain is a bit groggy, but the other part is awake. And I mean, I've even heard stories of lizards that will sleep, you know, with both eyes closed. But if they sense that there is a predator in the room, one eye opens and they switch to, you know, have a third sleep. So there's probably some flexibility, but a lot of mammals sleep in or birds will sleep in nests and they have sort of very safe habitats. And over time, maybe you have a unit of hemispheric sleep is not necessary anymore. And it's not as efficient as sleeping with both hemispheres. Right. It takes the dolphin twice as long to sleep because it have to do one side at a time. Okay. It's not just non-REM sleep that is due to hemisphere. REM sleep requires both hemispheres as far as we can tell. So dolphins have a hard time during REM sleep. Right. It's like they do. They have their little dream and then they have to breathe. Right. So that's that's tough. They're doing this at, you know, seconds or a minute at a time. They can hold their breath longer than us. Fortunately, when we get to the first seal, it's interesting because the first seal has a behaviour that's more like terrestrial mammals, but it also lives in the water. And it turns out that when it's in the water, you can't do very much REM sleep. And it also has to sleep with one hemisphere at a time. But as soon as it comes to land, then it says, okay, great. I don't need to consciously breathe, you know, underwater. I can have my REM sleep and I can sleep with both hemispheres and it switches. So it shows you two really important things. First, this is the this is much more efficient to sleep with both hemispheres and to have REM sleep. At the same time is not essential, right? It's hard to wrap your head around. How does an animal get away with not having REM sleep yet? As soon as it has an opportunity, it will do loads of it. Right. So it must have a function. We just don't know what it is. Okay. So to sum up so far, all animals with a brain need to sleep. As far as we know. If you discover an animal that doesn't need to sleep, nature paper, great sleep structure and requirements vary greatly across different species with different ethology, the habitat, the behaviour, all of that seems to influence how the animals sleep. So there is flexibility in what a species can do. It's not like I'm human. I have to sleep 8 hours. But it might take a long time for me to adapt to then sleep more or sleep less. So the big question is why is sleep important? So you have to look at sleep and say this is the worst possible idea evolution could have come up with. Right. What is your function as a species? It is to make more of you. To make more. Right. So mating. You need to get food for energy so you can do that. Okay. So foraging, hunting. And then there are other animals that are looking for food, too. And if they hunt you, you're not going to mate. Right. So you need to avoid predation and sleep is one third of your life and you can't do these essential things. So why would why would the brain evolve to take give you a disadvantage in these things? You must be getting something in return. But it's we still don't really understand what we are getting in return. And just to emphasise how important sleep is, right. You would probably say, well, you need food and water more. Right. Well, the world record for no water is 18 days. Okay. No food. 74 days. How long can someone go without sleep, you think? 11 days. Hey, Randy Gardner. Hey. Now it's even possible that part of his brain was sleeping. There are some reports of local sleep, but I think he had problems after this experiment that he performed on on himself. And you probably have tested yourself on going one day without sleep, maybe two. It's quite difficult. You don't function well. But I can tell you, as someone who has young kids, I went through a lot of sleep deprivation. You are not the same person. Okay. I'm still recovering. And they have done this as well on on rodents. And, you know, you put the animal basically on a on a dish that's rotating. So the animals not just have to move quickly, but it it's not continuously awake and where it goes into the water. And within 11 to 30 days or so, all the rats died on this experiment. Now, one very important thing one can found is that lack of sleep leads to stress. And stress has a huge effect on your mental mental health and well-being. Okay. So it's hard to know whether the ability to remove stress when that's gone, we are in trouble. And that's why we're having these problems. Or is it sleep itself? Okay. And it's also a vicious circle, as many of you probably realise that when you're stressed, you can't fall asleep. When you can't fall asleep. It will lead to more stress. Okay. So sleep is as important as mating, hunting, foraging for food, avoiding predation. But we don't know why. So some ideas. All of you have been sick at one point. What? What do you do? You go to your bed. You rest. You sleep. You sleep often more. So there are some benefits to your immune system and wound healing. Now, why that is special. To sleep and not just resting in bed. There seems to be your immune system does seem to be synchronised to your sleep cycle and there is probably a lot more there. But why that's the case. Why do you have to be unconscious for your immune system to kick in and act differently? We don't know. Second thing is energy conservation and protection. So many animals are adapted for hunting or foraging at night or during the day, but not both. Okay. So a good example is, you know, like the nocturnal primates that you find with these huge eyes. Right. Those are great. In Night-Time, they see much better than we do. But if you turn the light on, they're like, Oh, right. They would not be able to hunt. And in fact, they would be a danger of predation. So the best thing they can do is just hide right here during the day, hunt at night. But that still doesn't answer the question. Why do they have to be asleep? Why can't they just, you know, hang out in their their cubby-hole and just wait for the right time? The next idea is homeostasis. So when you're learning, hopefully today you're learning or your neurones are building new synapses, the synapses are getting stronger. But if you do that again and again and again, there might be some negative effects of scaling up your synapses too much. So during sleep, it's thought that there is a homeostatic mechanism that rebalances things, that things don't go too high or too low. And while still maintaining the changes that occurred during learning. Okay. Fourth is what's called the washing machine hypothesis that when you sleep, your brain is able to get rid of all the crap that's built up. Okay. So what do I mean by that? Well, if you look at amyloid beta, right, related to dementia, humans have a higher risk. Okay. If your sleep efficiency goes down. So if you're not sleeping well, you have more build-up of these toxins, which then can lead to dementia. Okay. And another example, you can look at how another thing related to Alzheimer's and there is a build-up of tell if someone is sleep deprived. So every time you're going out and partying all night, you're like, I don't need to sleep. I'm young, you're building up towel your brain, okay? You have a big capacity, you know, for how much you can have. But at some point you want to sleep and clear this stuff out so it doesn't build up over time. And it's not surprising that as you get older, sleep quality does decline. And these are associated with a lot of these sorts of problems. Okay. Now, if we look at the brain, this is with MRI, you can see that there are ways of activity happening during sleep. And when the activity goes up, okay, this happens slowly up and down when it goes Sorry, when it goes down. There is a CSF exchange which you see in blue. So activities in red, when the activity goes down, the CSF in the brain gets flushed out. And with this CSF, we think there are a lot of toxins that have built up and then it's sort of replenishes. So that's a bit like a washing machine, right? It's sort of flushing the brain out. And then you have new CSF filling up the region and this way you're getting rid of stuff. Okay. If you don't sleep, maybe this can still happen, but it seems that it's not as efficient. And there's something about sleep that is ideal for this to happen. Okay. So I talked to you about four hypotheses. The fifth one I'll focus on for the rest of the lecture, because that's what my lab does. And I know more about that than the other four. So the fifth hypothesis where there is a lot of evidence is that sleep is important for memory. And this idea that your recent memories need to get backed up to long term storage. And this has to happen during sleep. So there are lots of experiments you can find out in the literature where there is some sort of task. I mean, there are really dozens and dozens of these sorts of studies. On the y axis, you see that there's an improvement in the task. And if you've taken a nap after you do the task, you'll do better than if you didn't take a nap. Okay, so sleeping is beneficial At the same time, if you sleep deprived someone, you'll see that their memory retention is is not as good. So sleeping is good and not sleeping. When you're supposed to sleep, you do worse than you should. Okay. Now, as I said before, not sleeping leads to stress. And there are you know, there are other factors you have to examine. But even when you do these things, sleep seems to be better beneficial for your memory. So I told you that sleep is divided into REM sleep and non REM sleep and mammals and birds. So what is what do you think is more important for memory? Is it the REM sleep or the non REM sleep? Ram. Raise your hand if it's Ram. Raise your hand if it's non-REM. Okay. A couple brave souls. Good. So here's a task. I'll answer the question in a second. Here's a task where someone has to remember the location of different different dots on the screen. And while they're doing the task, they smell this odour in the background. Okay. I believe it's not a very good odour, but it's for the purpose of getting their attention and making that association. And then when they go to sleep, they can present that odour while they're sleeping. And the idea is because the olfactory bulb has direct connections to the campus, that smell is going to activate neurones and remind the brain, the hippocampus over here, which is active during sleep, reminding the hippocampus that, oh, this is what you were doing. Think about this memory and perhaps bias your your your memory processing towards the task rather than all the other things you did during the day. So it turns out if you do this during REM and waking, there is no effect at all on your memory. But if you do during slow sleep, if you present the odour, the performance improves after the sleep session compared to if there is no odour at all. Okay. So this doesn't mean that REM sleep is not important for memory. But it seems that slow wave sleep is where this might this process probably starts. Okay. The other thing is this works If you just have a nap. And a nap is almost entirely slow wave sleep. We're non-REM sleep. Okay. So you can do these control experiments that even without any REM sleep, you are benefiting from having having non-REM sleep. Okay. So what do you think is happening during this process? What? Why does this work? So. The brain is very complicated in lots of different areas. We're going to focus on the neocortex, which is, you know, the outside region, all your cortical areas for seeing and hearing and touch and the hippocampus, which is in your medial medial temporal lobe. Okay. Which is in blue over here. So it's thought that during non-REM sleep or sorry, prior to non-REM sleep, when you're awake, cortical activity is happening, which is representing your whole world. Right? It's very much like if I stimulate part of part of my visual cortex, I'll see a flash of light, right? It's not. What is actually happening out there, is what my brain is doing that gives me perception. So all these signals in my brain that are giving me my conscious experience are being transmitted to the hippocampus. And the hippocampus is recording this. Okay. And when you go to sleep during non-REM sleep, the hippocampus plays back all these memories. The cortex, of course, is not as active. It shouldn't be active at all from the outside world because your eyes are shut and you're unconscious, but your brain is actually still very active and your hippocampus talks to you in your cortex and said, Hey, this is what you do during the day. Let's talk about it. Okay. And the analogy is that it's very much like your computer. You download content during the day when your computer is on, you're working on it, and then you put your computer in sleep mode and everything gets backed up to the cloud. Okay. And you have these two modes. When the computer is active and you're doing stuff, it's going in one direction. When your computer is in sleep mode, it can still be active and can still do stuff when you don't need the computer. And it uses that opportunity to back up the data if it did it while you're working at least, well, maybe now computers are much faster. But back in my day, it would slow things down, right? So it's better to have these two modes. So what is the evidence that this is happening? All of you know H.M. I don't need to go through the story. But he had his hippocampus removed to try to stop the seizures. And when I said the campus was removed, he suffered from anterior grade and temporally graded retrograde amnesia for episodic and semantic memory, which meant he couldn't form new memories, and his old memories were partially affected. The further back in time those memories were initially encoded, the better his memory was, Which means the new experiences require the hippocampus and hippocampal damage does not erase old memories. So if a computer example we just had. If I take my computer. Right. And I don't know, it just blows the fuse. Doesn't work anymore. Okay. Everything. I backed up to the cloud I still have access to that's still there. I can't store new memories. I can't download content. I don't have a computer anymore, but I still have access to the old memories. And that's what we think the hippocampus is, is for. Taking new memories and storing it somewhere else. So the hippocampus is gone. You just can't do it again. You can't store new memories. Okay. Now, that's in humans. What happens when we look at rats, right? We can study neural signals in the brain. These are experiments I do in my lab. This video is not from my lab, but we record. We put electrodes. Many, many electrodes to record 5000 neurones at the same time. And when neurones, when you're listening to neurones, they act very much like instruments in an orchestra. They're all doing their thing. And when they produce the melody together, it's very easy to recognise they form patterns. And the question is what are those patterns there they're forming? What do those patterns mean? So. Okay, so you can hear the sound. Isn't the sounds of the spikes when you're recording. What you're seeing is a colour code over here of the different spikes. Okay. And what you find is that when the animals and when a particular neurone is is responding, it likes a particular region of the track, the animals running. So I know I talked about the hippocampus as being important for memory, and this will come full circle to that. But when you're recording in the Rat, it seems like these neurones act like a GPS. And so the neurones like one location, another neurone will like a different location. And as the animal is running, you have neurone A and B and C and D firing. Okay. And I'll give you a little cartoon over here. Here's animal running. And you see the first cell fire, then the the second, the third, the fourth. And if you were listening to the brain and you saw blue, green, red, orange, you say, are the animals running along this track? Hey, I know what the animal is doing. But then the animal goes to sleep and you see the same neurones fire in the same sequence. This is a phenomena called replay. It happens much faster, but it's in the same sequence. And because this sequence is very unique to both the track and animal's behaviour and can decode this and say, we know the animal is thinking about the track and its sleep. I don't know if this is a dream. You can't ask the animal what it's what's the conscious experience. Right. But we can see these patterns are very unique. They don't happen by chance very easily. And this seems to reflect what has happened. So just so you can hear how that sounds. And remember, that replay event is 5 to 20 times faster. So it sounds like all the neurones are fired at once. We'll look at the video again. You're going to see a neurone fire on the bottom, in the middle, on the left side, on the track. And when it gets to the corner all the way on the left, one is rounding. That's where the replay event will occur. And I'll I'll tell you when that happens. So. So it's running the blue cell fires. And another the science cell. Another blue cell. And here, that was a replay event. Okay. And if you saw the cell activity that we play, there are actually cells active in front of the animal where it's planning to go. Okay. Now. So the animal is not asleep here. Okay. This happens when the animal's awake as well, when it's sort of tuned out and not running. You see these replay events, but when the animal goes to sleep and you're listening to the brain, you hear, Shh, shh, shh. And these are all replay events. Okay. Yes. Dear me while I was. Is the replay as fast. Yeah. So when it's asleep and when it's awake. There are probably differences in the replay, but we don't really understand those differences. But the speed is similar. During REM sleep. There's one study finding replay. Okay. And it's the same speed as the actual experience. So maybe this is closer to what you have as a as a as a dream. The other thing is that during slow wave, sleep is very fragmented. So it's like part of the track boom animal replays. And then one second later, another part of the track, boom, there's replay. Okay. So it's not this. Imagine a dream is you have this whole storyline that doesn't seem to be happening here. Maybe REM sleep does. Okay. So you have a behavioural episode. Animals running along, you know, looking for food on the track. This is all getting encoded in your cortex. It's getting sent to the hippocampus to store. And then when the animal is not paying attention, you know, sort of zoning out or sleeping. We call these offline periods and there's a process of memory consolidation where the hippocampus backs up this information, the cortex. And one reason for this is that the hippocampus learns things quickly. Okay. And it has to decide what are the most important things to store. You can't store everything you experience in your brain. The hippocampus has to make that choice. So one thing we study in the lab is a phenomena of memory triage. Just. Just like a Annie, right? You can't help everyone. You have to rank. What are the most important cases to see first? So what are the most important memories? And during sleep, those get replayed. So what happens if we disrupt replay so we can do this by putting electrodes in the brain near the anterior comissioner? So the connection between the hippocampi and when we see this high oscillation, which is called a sharp wave ripple where replay events occur, we'd give a little pulse and disrupts neural activity for 100 or 200 milliseconds in the brain. And if we time it right, we prevent replay events from occurring. Okay. So we can train the animal on a memory task involving different arms that are baited. And when we disrupt replay, we find that the rate of learning is slower in those animals. They're still able to learn, but the rate is slower. And you have to keep in mind that you're blocking replay just for an hour after the task, and then the animal can sleep in its own cage after that normally. So you're able to disrupt some, but it's not the perfect experiment where you've blocked all replay that will ever occur, you know, during the sleep session. But this is already a hint that replay is important for learning a memory. So an experiment that I did a while back rather than disruption, where you always wonder, maybe I have screwed something up in the brain by shocking. Can we do something that is positive? And so where we improve behaviour or we can bias activity towards certain memories. So I train rats on a task where they had to run to one side if they heard a high frequency sound, like high notes on the piano, and they ran to the other side or they heard lower frequency sounds okay. So they can do the task and it takes a while. Humans are a bit faster. Rats can learn this in a week to two weeks, but they learn. Hey, I hear the sound around this. I hear that sound runs the other side and they go to sleep. And what you would expect if you're recording brain activity is you would see replay of running to the right and replay of running to the left right. These are relevant memories for the animal to learn. It was getting a reward. It has to figure out the task. So you're going to see these replay events. Now, the question is, if I play sounds associated with the task, the sound telling the animal to the left, the sound telling the animal to go right. Does that affect the replay? Right. Just with the like, the old factory cue I showed you before that influenced the memory. So potentially, since the brain is still able to hear sounds, maybe not in exactly the same way you still hear sound when you're sleeping. Maybe I can sort of tease the hippocampus to replay what I wanted to replay. And that's what I found, that you can you can have a measure called the mean rate bias, which is basically you have more activity for these neurones. If the neurone likes running to the right. Are those neurones more active for sound? Are compared to sound. The sound telling it to go right. So the blue bar is higher, which means the neurones are more than neurones unlike to run right respond in animal runs. Right are more active when you play sound. Ah and the neurones that like going left are less active when you play sound are. So there seems to be a preference for what replays when you play a sound. And what's interesting is that this bias over here persists. So you might think I play a sound. I get a replay of just like that. That's not what happens. Okay. You play the sound and you create a state in the brain for almost 10 seconds where if a replay event occurs, it is more likely to be in the direction that is associated with that sound. Okay. So you're not causing more replay events. You were just biasing what will happen when a replay event occurs? But as soon as you play another sound, the fact goes away. So it seems to be very, very intolerant to say lots of different things you're doing. Supplying a sound cue caused a greater reactivation of play cells associated with that sound for more than 10 seconds, which was the max tested. So the summary so far is if you train the animal to run left for. A high pitched sound and run rate for a low pitched sound. Playing the high pitched sound will cause replays that left at a low pitch Sound will cause replay to the right. If you do this in humans, humans, you don't have to train them to run right or run less for just two sounds. You can do things that are more complicated so you can have 50 objects. Each object is the subject has to remember the location the object should be. So they're touching on the screen. Oh, I see the cattle. I need to press this part of the screen. And each object is also paired with a sound. Okay, so you have 50 pictures, sound pairings that the subject has to remember and during a nap. So that's only slow wave sleep. They present half the sounds to the subjects while they're sleeping. Okay, So the sounds they present are called the cued stimuli and the unseen stimuli are they still have a sound, right? But those sounds were not played. Then they test the subjects with acute versus on cued. Okay. Importantly, they do not present the sound in the test. It's only the visual objects and the person has to remember the location. And when they do this they see there is less errors in the cued sound. So there is learning, there is an improvement in the cued versus on cued. So presenting a sound during sleep from the right literature suggest that we can actually bias what is replaying in the brain. And then on top of that, in humans, if you have presumably more replay of one memory, you have a better memory than if you're replaying it less. Okay. So. What's the take home message when you study? Maybe you should have some sounds in the background. I don't know how robust this is for normal sleep conditions outside the lab, but if you have test specific sounds, not music per se, but test specific sounds so like you have a word you have to remember for, you know, anatomy. And then you say, okay, every time I'm thinking about this word, I'm going to hear the sound that if you present that sound when you're sleeping, you'll probably remember the word pairing better. And so if yes, if you present those sounds while you're sleeping, you should have a better test performance than if you just sleep on it on your own. Now, even more important, if you don't sleep, your performance will definitely be worse. Okay. If there is one take home message, you need to sleep. And. This. This leads us to evidence that more replay leads to better memories. This phenomenon of replay, and that's something that we study in the lab now of how do you manipulate the amount of replay in a rats or mouse? And how does this change the performance of the animal? Okay. So thanks for your attention. I have time for a few questions, but. |