File size: 45,549 Bytes
dae990d |
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846 847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863 864 865 866 867 868 869 870 871 872 873 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 883 884 885 886 887 888 889 890 891 892 893 894 895 896 897 898 899 900 901 902 903 904 905 906 907 908 909 910 911 912 913 914 915 916 917 918 919 920 921 922 923 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 933 934 935 936 937 938 939 940 941 942 943 944 945 946 947 948 949 950 951 952 953 954 955 956 957 958 959 960 961 962 963 964 965 966 967 968 969 970 971 972 973 974 975 976 977 978 979 980 981 982 983 984 985 986 987 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 995 996 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 1003 1004 1005 1006 1007 1008 1009 1010 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1020 1021 1022 1023 1024 1025 1026 1027 1028 1029 1030 1031 1032 1033 1034 1035 1036 1037 1038 1039 1040 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047 1048 1049 1050 1051 1052 1053 1054 1055 1056 1057 1058 1059 1060 1061 1062 1063 1064 1065 1066 1067 1068 1069 1070 1071 1072 1073 1074 1075 1076 1077 1078 1079 1080 1081 1082 1083 1084 1085 1086 1087 1088 1089 1090 1091 1092 1093 1094 1095 1096 1097 1098 1099 1100 1101 1102 1103 1104 1105 1106 1107 1108 1109 1110 1111 1112 1113 1114 1115 1116 1117 1118 1119 1120 1121 1122 1123 1124 1125 1126 1127 1128 1129 1130 1131 1132 1133 1134 1135 1136 1137 1138 1139 1140 1141 1142 1143 1144 1145 1146 1147 1148 1149 1150 1151 1152 1153 1154 1155 1156 1157 1158 1159 1160 1161 1162 1163 1164 1165 1166 1167 1168 1169 1170 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1180 1181 1182 1183 1184 1185 1186 1187 1188 1189 1190 1191 1192 1193 1194 1195 1196 1197 1198 1199 1200 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1210 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1220 1221 1222 1223 1224 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1230 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 1240 1241 1242 1243 1244 1245 1246 1247 1248 1249 1250 1251 1252 1253 1254 1255 1256 1257 1258 1259 1260 1261 1262 1263 1264 1265 1266 1267 1268 1269 1270 1271 1272 1273 1274 1275 1276 1277 1278 1279 1280 1281 1282 1283 1284 1285 1286 1287 1288 1289 1290 1291 1292 1293 1294 1295 1296 1297 1298 1299 1300 1301 1302 1303 1304 1305 1306 1307 1308 1309 1310 1311 1312 1313 1314 1315 1316 1317 1318 1319 1320 1321 1322 1323 1324 1325 1326 1327 1328 1329 1330 1331 1332 1333 1334 1335 1336 1337 1338 1339 1340 1341 1342 1343 1344 1345 1346 1347 1348 1349 1350 1351 1352 1353 1354 1355 1356 1357 1358 1359 1360 1361 1362 1363 1364 1365 1366 1367 1368 1369 1370 1371 1372 1373 1374 1375 1376 1377 1378 1379 1380 1381 1382 1383 1384 1385 1386 1387 1388 1389 1390 1391 1392 1393 1394 1395 1396 1397 1398 1399 1400 1401 1402 1403 1404 1405 1406 1407 1408 1409 1410 1411 1412 1413 1414 1415 1416 1417 1418 1419 1420 1421 1422 1423 1424 1425 1426 1427 1428 1429 1430 1431 1432 1433 1434 1435 1436 1437 1438 1439 1440 1441 1442 1443 1444 1445 1446 1447 1448 1449 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 1485 1486 1487 1488 1489 1490 1491 1492 1493 1494 1495 1496 1497 1498 1499 1500 1501 1502 1503 1504 1505 1506 1507 1508 1509 1510 1511 1512 1513 1514 1515 1516 1517 1518 1519 1520 1521 1522 1523 1524 1525 1526 1527 1528 1529 1530 1531 1532 1533 1534 1535 1536 1537 1538 1539 1540 1541 1542 1543 1544 1545 1546 1547 1548 1549 1550 1551 1552 1553 1554 1555 1556 1557 1558 1559 1560 1561 1562 1563 1564 1565 1566 1567 1568 1569 1570 1571 1572 1573 1574 1575 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 1581 1582 1583 1584 1585 1586 1587 1588 1589 1590 1591 1592 1593 1594 1595 1596 1597 1598 1599 1600 1601 1602 1603 1604 1605 1606 1607 1608 1609 1610 1611 1612 1613 1614 1615 1616 1617 1618 1619 1620 1621 1622 1623 1624 1625 1626 1627 1628 1629 1630 1631 1632 1633 1634 1635 1636 1637 1638 1639 1640 1641 1642 1643 1644 1645 1646 1647 1648 1649 1650 1651 1652 1653 1654 1655 1656 1657 1658 1659 1660 1661 1662 1663 1664 1665 1666 1667 1668 1669 1670 1671 1672 1673 1674 1675 1676 1677 1678 1679 1680 1681 1682 1683 1684 1685 1686 1687 1688 1689 1690 1691 1692 1693 1694 1695 1696 1697 1698 1699 1700 1701 1702 1703 1704 1705 1706 1707 1708 1709 1710 1711 1712 1713 1714 1715 1716 1717 1718 1719 1720 1721 1722 1723 1724 1725 1726 1727 1728 1729 1730 1731 1732 1733 1734 1735 1736 1737 1738 1739 1740 1741 1742 1743 1744 1745 1746 1747 1748 1749 1750 1751 1752 1753 1754 1755 1756 1757 1758 1759 1760 1761 1762 1763 1764 1765 1766 1767 1768 1769 1770 1771 1772 1773 1774 1775 1776 1777 1778 1779 1780 1781 1782 1783 1784 1785 1786 1787 1788 1789 1790 1791 1792 1793 1794 1795 1796 1797 1798 1799 1800 1801 1802 1803 1804 1805 1806 1807 1808 1809 1810 1811 1812 1813 1814 1815 1816 1817 1818 1819 1820 1821 1822 1823 1824 1825 1826 1827 1828 1829 1830 1831 1832 1833 1834 1835 1836 1837 1838 1839 1840 1841 1842 1843 1844 1845 1846 1847 1848 1849 1850 1851 1852 1853 1854 1855 1856 1857 1858 1859 1860 1861 1862 1863 1864 1865 1866 1867 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874 1875 1876 1877 1878 1879 1880 1881 1882 1883 1884 1885 1886 1887 1888 1889 1890 1891 1892 1893 1894 1895 1896 1897 1898 1899 1900 1901 1902 1903 1904 1905 1906 1907 1908 1909 1910 1911 1912 1913 1914 1915 1916 1917 1918 1919 1920 1921 1922 1923 1924 1925 1926 1927 1928 1929 1930 1931 1932 1933 1934 1935 1936 1937 1938 1939 1940 1941 1942 1943 1944 1945 1946 1947 1948 1949 1950 1951 1952 1953 1954 1955 1956 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 1976 1977 1978 1979 1980 1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015 2016 2017 2018 2019 2020 2021 2022 2023 2024 2025 2026 2027 2028 2029 2030 2031 2032 2033 2034 2035 2036 2037 2038 2039 2040 2041 2042 2043 2044 2045 2046 2047 2048 2049 2050 2051 2052 2053 2054 2055 2056 2057 2058 2059 2060 2061 2062 2063 2064 2065 2066 2067 2068 2069 2070 2071 2072 2073 2074 2075 2076 2077 2078 2079 2080 2081 2082 2083 2084 2085 2086 2087 2088 2089 2090 2091 2092 2093 2094 2095 2096 2097 2098 2099 2100 2101 2102 2103 2104 2105 2106 2107 2108 2109 2110 2111 2112 2113 2114 2115 2116 2117 2118 2119 2120 2121 2122 2123 2124 2125 2126 2127 2128 2129 2130 2131 2132 2133 2134 2135 2136 2137 2138 2139 2140 2141 |
Good morning. So welcome back. Friday. Monday. We heard about stress. Today we're going to talk about social bonding. Before we kick off, I just want to remind everyone about while you're doing this module, do keep checking your anatomy. So this is a page in the anatomy and your guide. So here's the web guide from lecture two. There's a whole load of brain sections to memorise. And here is where one of them goes by halfway through, about halfway through this module. Now a bit more. So you keep playing with this. Can you tell some easy ones in this? Where's the cerebellum? Where's the corpus callosum? Where's the military bodies? That's much harder, right? So here's where it says Yeah. What number? Three bodies. So if you thought that was the military bodies, well done. But this is the way. Just keep testing yourself. Where's the midbrain? Where's the pons? Where is the body? Keep looking through this material. As you go through this course, I'm going to provide some new anatomy today. That isn't the place to do with the hypothalamus picking up on last week. That story on Monday's lecture. Okay. So let's go to our slides. So Monday's talk on stressed is quite negative most of the time. Stress is a part of our life. You going to have to sit in exam for this module. That's inherently stressful. But another key way in which our way in which we deal with stress is part of that is our friendship groups, our parents, our sisters and brothers. All potentially killing all these people around us really help as humans mitigate stress and other primates and some of the species. Here's an example of two types of what we might call social bonding. A parent with a child or two people who have couple together romantically in the textbooks. And the research on this topic of how do these bonds form between these two groups. This is known as affiliation, and we'll dive into that in this lecture. So there are many ways in which bonds form between people of different species. On the left at the top, down the top left is humans holding hands. In many cultures around the world, that's a way of single signifying bonding. Swans have an amazing kind of approach with their heads. And I think the cutest one of all really is the two old world monkeys who twine their tails together. So these are the monkeys bond for life as partners. And when they do that, they sit together with their tails bound up. You can go and see them in London Zoo. They have, or at least the last time where the London Zoo in the tropical house, they have two world monkeys. So these are these are some of the things we see, but we don't there are groups studying humans or a group studying monkeys, small number of groups. But a lot of the time research has been done on these animals is not rats, but actually voles. Those turn out to be a fascinating story in terms of our understanding of social bonding. And for humans, what social bonding bonding is linked to is love, the love we have for our parents, our family, our friends, our partner, etc., and our children. We don't know if these animals experience love, but they have very similar biochemical effects in their brains to us. And because we can study those in more detail, we can understand that. So I talk about social bonding in this lecture, but the technical term is affiliation that encompasses the bonds between individuals, partners who come together to produce children and the parent parents that bond. And look at the general process of that bonding in the beginning part of this lecture and turn to the parental bonds at the end. So this I mentioned this this coupling up the social bonding promotes adoption to the world. You can cope better when you've got friends, to put it another way, and stress, the social isolation which doesn't occur is linked to a whole range of psychiatric disorders. So it's an important endeavour in our society to improve social bonding, really. So here's the more of a longer and a bit. One of the key things you need to learn in this lecture is the word called specific to the ability to recognise a specific. So this is another species of the same of another individual of your same species. So as humans walking around, you can detect humans. So you as the human, the most dangerous thing out there for humans and most of their animals. So you really do need to detect humans, but also the most useful animal you probably interact with animals, I imagine the vast majority of you. So you need to be able to do that. You don't really need to be able to recognise as a human, but which humanism? Who is it I'm recognising and then do I need to remember to maintain a relationship with this person? So this all comes under this bonding process of not only do tend to find that is that is the human. I do know that human and I do need to invest time with that humans. So the there are a number of tasks that have been used. That's all fine in theory, but how do we test that? One is to look at approach. So here are two dogs. Dogs love to approach each other. Are people that dog owners know this. They love to get in and sniff each other's bottoms. They do more than that. They really get in there and get in there with them with the urine and material to understand. They do more than we do as humans. They also need to learn to recognise and said who. And there's a lot of work which cheap, cheap but very, very similar to us humans. But they have to identify the individual sheep they're interacting with. And again, that investment with sheep, how does the sheep know its mother and how does the mother know which individual lamb is its lamb to take care of? And there's been research on that. Now, how a lot of those animals do that is through smell of action. So in red highlighted that humans are a bit unusual in other primates, that our visual system is so good we can see people far off and we can hear thing. We can use that visual information to guide our social interactions. Not all other animals share that ability, and what they do is use of actions that ferments. You may be aware from popular culture are floating in the air. These are these are gas tight released chemicals that have some role in humans, but most are important for other species in terms of their their interrelationships. But beyond you know, beyond that, we have a particular organ in the brains of most vertebrates. So we have here reptiles and mammals that's shown by rats and a snake, and they have what's known as a bomb or a nasal organ. It detects in the air these, these pheromones I can relay them to. A particular bit of the brain illustrates that in both species called the accessory olfactory bulbs. So if you're smelling like a beautiful perfume, you'll likely be using your main olfactory system to detect that process that, Oh, this is lovely and it passes through your factory system. Separate to that main pathway is the access, the real factory bulbs, and they're helpful for detecting those social cues in these. So they pass that information that's shown here to the thalamus, that campus and the amygdala to various other bits of the brain that we'll come see. So this will really keep it to the brain to do that. Social detection, Who is this? And that's what we're looking for, the vulnerable nasal organism, very much in action in that picture. The two dogs, humans don't seem to have a formal gaze. Morgana starts to develop in the womb when you're a tiny baby and then gets regressed, it becomes removed. You don't end up coming out of the womb with the vulnerable base puts a set of eyes focussed on it. So the how do we test that? Once we've got that detection, how do we how do we explore that So it rodents and that's voles and rats and mice. And one approach is to take the top the two to the top one is to take that rat, expose it to another rat to play with. The two rats will play with each other a bit. I spent time in a board and run away into it during sniffing and grooming. If you put them back together a little bit later, don't just ignore each other again. They remember, Oh, it's this right? But if they're given a new rat again, explore and extending, you know, sniff each other and greet each other, and all those things that rats do when they interact. So you can measure that systematically how much times these two animals interact with each other and use that as a measure of social recognition. So if you were to damage the ability to do social recognition, a rat can't do that. We just treat all rats as if they knew they won't trust, will not know who it's mated with, who is it's daughters or sons or in terms of rat pups. It would just it wouldn't have that ability. So that's one way to measure it more. Find ways to use a social discrimination procedure that's been used extensively in bowls. So put the test participant in the neutral middle chamber with door flaps that can run through doorways into other chambers. What they'll do is have one. Another animal on one of these is tethered, is a little tether around its legs. They can't escape that chamber. Just briefly, for the purposes of a short experiment and another chamber with another one with a little tether. That's what shown here holding. It's like they can't get into the middle chamber, an experiment test whether the devil in the middle will spend more time with the partner than it's mated with recently might be. Or it might be. It could test lots of things, but typically it might be a partner that the vole has mated with or obviously spent a lot of time with to invest the time with or completely unknown vole that it doesn't know, or it could just hang out in the middle. But what this this fascinating work, there's a review we recommend you read in the reading material and so in young which really explored all of this work was there are two two different distinct species that probably more but two distinct species of vole, a montane voles, a prairie voles. There they look almost identical, but they have different genetic backgrounds and different environments. Now what you can see is this time spent in each of the chambers and we've colour coded these. So the green period is the partner one, the neutral one is the sort of purple one and the beige one is the stranger. So what you can see for Montane Voles is they don't. Let this go in the middle is just mated with this other one, but it would rather spend time on its own. It's just going to spend time sitting here grooming, maybe eating and doing what it wants to do, but it doesn't really distinguish between the two other goals. That's not true of prairie voles. They're less likely than anywhere to be in the neutral chamber, and they're very late to spend time with a stranger. What they spend most that time is with the partner they've made up with each other. That's helpful. So this shows these two different species that look very similar, have very different social behaviours. And as you can see in the wild, these, these variables will mate and the bond and they'll spend time together and they'll snuggle up. If I go right back to this early picture. That's two prairie voles together with their offspring. And they really co-invest and raise those offspring together. So that's one way. So what's different with these these voles? What's different between montane and prairie voles? And this review goes into this in a lot of detail. And so for this lecture and we've got 40 minutes, I'm just going to highlight a key takeaway message, and that is when you look at the brains of Montane voles and prairie voles is a particular molecule or two particular molecules that appear to be different. Oxytocin, the vasopressin, come on to explain those in the next slide. So these two molecules are much more abundant. They have many more receptors in key areas of the brain for motivation in prairie voles than they do in montane voles. So what's going on? So they discovered this. What's going on? Why these molecules? Well, the next experimental thing is for scientists to do is let's manipulate that. What if we were to inject these voles with oxytocin? What happens if we boost it artificially? Or what if we knock it out by giving a drug that stops them working? A lot of that painstaking work over decades of research, lots of graphs, statistics, publications, somebody just put it together in a cartoon slide to explain a lot of billions of US dollars being spent. What you're looking at the top is two voles. And what brought to this picture to me, but they're mainly work is work is on voles. And what they'll do is what this thing at the top is. Normally this slide would be even clearer, is that typically the two would be the thing with love with each other for the diagram. But the top one, the male profile has been injected with very suppresses and he hasn't before mating. So the two the two goals here typically ignore each other. In this first instance, they don't know each other that strangers. If we go back to that previous slide, this is this state here. The stranger doesn't care, the verbal. But what they've done in this experiment is inject visa pressing into the male. Now he keeps trying to snuggle up to the female, but what does she do? She runs off. She doesn't know. She's not interested in it. And so that's what this diagram, this this illustration is sort of looking away. What is this male sorry to do here? What if they inject oxytocin into the female? You get the same effect the other way round. Now the female is trying to couple up with the male, but the male is confused and running off. Normally, if they mated together, they would both show higher vasopressin oxytocin levels, but they're not. So this showing that you can boost this bonding process just by injecting one chemical that turns out to be very depressing for male voles and oxytocin for female voles. They're different. They work slightly different in these two species and humans. It's a more mixed picture. It isn't that simple for other animals, but for voles, it turns out to be these two, this to just this way. Right? So the problem here is that's the period before. This is about confusing the animals, artificially boosting bonding when it hasn't happened down at the bottom is saying they have mated. Now, these two, those should have enough eyes for each other and be snuggled up. And that's indeed what the female is doing after mating here. But they've injected an antagonist, a blocker agent pervasive present in the male and that he's not wanting to couple up and bond. So they've turned off his natural inclination to snuggle up with the female and invest time with her just by injecting one chemical change this entire behaviour and then they find they can do the exact same thing with the female just using oxytocin antagonist. They've blocked oxytocin after meeting in the female and now she's sort of in this illustration telling him he's a loser and he should get lost in this illustration to highlight what the scientists are inferring because is much more biological than then. To this, what's going on? Let's turn to the anatomy now. Let's turn to the human brain. The brain is sitting at this lecture theatre in front of me. And so here's a here's a sagittal section through the human brain. You can see the pons and the midbrain and so on. And if we zoom in on this bit underneath the corpus callosum, we have the thalamus in the middle here. Here we have a region of the brain underneath the thalamus called the hypothalamus, and we learned about it in lecture last Monday going on stress. We talked about the anterior pituitary previously as the output of the stress hormones, a hormone that goes in activates the adrenal cortical to growth hormone from the anterior. Now we're going to look at the posterior pituitary. The anterior is involved in stress. The posterior is involved in that building process. So what the scientists discovered, looking at the voles and then exploring in humans, is that there's a particular nucleus, the power of ventricular nucleus. It's very similar in your brain as a mammal to evolved, very similar. It just sits next to a bit ventricles. That's the reason that gets his name. There's also a super optic nucleus just above the optic plasma. That's where it gets its name from. So these two nuclei have nothing to do with ventricles or optic fibre optic pathways. It's just the name comes from where they're located in the brain, but they're tiny nuclei. They have more than five cells. This illustrations obviously schematic, but these are these are the bodies with cells that act with dendrites and they have axons, a good tone and an interface onto the blood cell, the blood, the circulation system, the capillaries. And they release, as is shown here, oxytocin, invasive breast and into the bloodstream from the Stewart the posterior pituitary gland. So typically this is what's known as neuroendocrine communication. So here we have our classic sounds of them and talked about in lecture three about sign ups, this and the transmitter systems. You also have endocrine responses in your body where you have hormones that operate here, like puberty is driven by our hormones. Here. What we're talking about is this other third pathway where you've got neurones that are connecting into the blood and releasing. This is known as neuroendocrine function. And that's how this this process is occurring. Now, this slide takes us on the journey from voles in a lab and injecting them to humans rather than injecting humans. They will take a nasal infusion of oxytocin into people's nose and get it through circulating up into the brain. And then what they found is that there's a whole range of different experiments. But one is that to show that oxytocin changes the way in which we treat faces, it's been a bit controversial. There's, you know, trying to get these expressed. Replicate can vary because not everyone reacts. Some people are really good at facial detection and others not so good. And these individual differences can be a challenge. But overall, the evidence weights the oxytocin and upregulate your ability for social processing. So detecting people, treating a neutral face is a slightly more positive game. Experiments where you have to trust other people to solve problems apparently are sort of elevated by oxytocin. But here's an experiment shown here by DOMS without biological psychiatry where they injected a placebo, so nothing going into the body or oxytocin or not injected these infusion again. And you can see, going back to Solomon's lectures, the amygdala responses to angry faces in the placebo condition is really high. There's an emotional response basically to these against the neutral faces. But what you see is that this is dampened so you get less reactivity, too fearful or angry, but in fact happy faces. So the whole thing sort of reactivity in the amygdala is lowered. That means that you're more likely to approach people, you're more likely to engage in activities of behaviours. That bonding process is what's argued by these authors. And there's a review in 2009 in Frontiers in neuro and chronology. So just to take that from a lab experiment, injecting people in the lab looking at brain scans is a real world example like I gave in the Fitbit on Monday. This is a couple of Nick Fleming and Linda Gaddis, who had their blood molecules, had their blood circulation taken. Here's Linda. Having her blood extracted on her wedding day, consented to a centrifuge and examined out her wedding alongside her husband, their parents, for their close friends and various people. And this is a wedding I actually attended. So is that what you're going to write it up for? The Daily Mail? So this became known as the cuddle, the cuddle chemical oxytocin in the study. And what is this? This just highlights the kind of thing that would occur. And this is to give a real life example. A month later, after the wedding, Linda got an email from Zach, the research scientist. And to her delight, the predictions were correct over oxytocin. Her husband Nick, and her experienced rise in the the love hormone with the cuddle chemical or oxytocin during the ceremony and the mother of the bride, the father and the relatives, they all boosted. But the friends were more mixed to experience the rise. But five didn't. Perhaps they were not feeling the love. So that's the way the Daily Mail has covered this kind of research. And you should be sceptical. Be careful about overinterpreting. This is the love molecule. It's a molecule that is raised and you can see that that actually does fit the predictions that these these molecules will rise in a small sample in a very particular scenario. But it's a very well studied phenomenon. The rise in oxytocin relates to social bonding, but it does more than that. So there's a really nice article called Oxytocin, the Great Facilitator of Life. And we're looking at this period here, social recognition, and we've been talking about social bonding and mate choice and feeling of trust and recognising social partners and children involved in play, but it's also involved in a whole lot of things that enable, as you can see here, through sexual behaviour, dampening aggression, potentially unintended childbirth, raising children, tiny babies through lactation and improper parenting is the mother right through. So they've argued this this whole cycle of life or maintenance as a species is somewhat dependent on oxytocin functioning. But it's not the only one like oxytocin works with others. There's things like serotonin and adrenaline. To all these molecules you need. Oxytocin appears to play this great, facilitative process that improves and enhances all these things. So that's the first part of the lecture I'm now going to focus. I've been talking about the bonding process here. I'm not going to look at this bit parenting, what happens in maternal and paternal behaviours. There's a really great review. It's quite sure maybe four pages by Dulac and colleagues in science from 2014 on the Moodle page is also a short 15 minute video. The Catherine du Lac is one of the best researchers in the world on this. Just talks through the camera about parenting process and what she's uncovered in her work and various prestigious institutes and what they've been able to do. It is absolutely remarkable when we dive into this work. So what they've shown in this work is that parenting occurs in a surprisingly large variety of vertebrates and invertebrates insects, arachnids, molluscs, fish recipients, reptiles, birds and of course, mammals. We've been talking about I've been talking about mammals all the way through here. But you have beetles that carry around their young. You have frogs that occur after they're young, you have birds and of course birds. And the most classic thing is birds. They have to look after their nests with their eggs in it. They have to pay it. Young, bring them food when they hatch. And so so there's a lot of analysis of parenting. The most common form of parenting is its female unit, parental. That means that the female is giving birth to the young and takes the majority of the role in raising that young. The male may lost. She's responsible and that accounts for that. And that's, as noted here, to carry the offspring. She has them. But there are many species that show this by parental care. Birds are amazing. When 90% of birds are shown in examples here, both parents are involved in raising the young. And that's partly to do with the one of them sitting on the eggs and looking after them. But the challenge of bringing food in for their offspring means the birds are particularly, you know, we'll spend a lot of co-investment. But that's also true of snakes and reptiles in some cases. Then they share this. But there are also some species of bird important to be aware of where there's a male unique parents. Also stickleback fish or a good example. And seahorses are not shown here, but they show particular male that the female will lay the eggs, but the male will then take over the role of raising those young to the point at which they're they grow up and become independent. So it's quite fascinating. So what's what's the takes on this new lots of different species here. But one of the conclusions of studying them in Europe biologically is that there appear to be across beetles to the to the stickleback fish through the reptiles and amphibians here, all the way through birds and mammals at the top are highly conserved, antagonistic circuits. What I mean by antagonistic these things don't work together. They are on or off. You can't be doing this and that. They are antagonistic in controlling either the activity of social bonding, parental behaviour, or aggressive behaviour towards offspring. And adult animals can display parental care or aggression according to their physiological state. Are they ready to do that? And what is the environment? So it's a combination of that. So these are these are statements. These are course conclusions that come from that review, and we'll dive into those in a moment. But it's worth being aware, we live in quite a civilised vertical society that if you step out of the door, you're not going to see a huge amount of aggression, I hope, today. But I have to go back more than 200 years and you would see a lot of aggression in humans and there is a lot of aggression in other parts of the world. Very sadly, there are wars going on. We are quite violent species. But that's also true of a lot of other mammals and other species. There's a lot of fighting for survival out there, and that requires that fighting and obtaining the the nutrients and the food and water you need requires aggression. You have to fight for things, for survival that Lucian has built into the brain of these mammals and other other vertebrates. Aggressive behaviour to survive. But whether they're aggressive or caring depends on the situation. So this is a graph from that paper that'll take a little while to unpack. So what we can see here and explain the y axis, first of all, this y axis, and this comes from a this review. As I said, here's some quick actions. If you look at the Y axis is what percentage of a group of male laboratory mice, these are males who are virgin as and they're not they're not mated previously. And they're previously this is this is a graph after those male cf1 strain mice have first and they're like mated. So they have mated. They're not going to have offspring. And what it shows you in the initial period before or just after that is the natural behaviour. This is what most male male mice will do, and this is quite similar to a lot of other rodent species and other mammals. Lions, for example, BE The classic example is that most of the time, given the opportunity, if that mice finds another unprotected pup in other mice, it will attack it, it'll destroy it. It's, it's trying to unless it's a pup, it won't, it will treat it with attack. It might ignore it in many cases. Occasionally it might retrieve that pup. There might be social circumstances under which it gets activated to help retrieve that problem. But you can see just in the spirit this fluctuates from here. 80% of all the male mice to looking at Habitat, the men being given this experiment just a few days after mating. But then there's a radical shift in these male mice. His brain. Something is switching off that aggressive circuit, and it dropped to 10% of them are attacking and 80% of them are switching to retrieving pups. That can't be their own pups because they've just mated. But it will be other pups they find scattered in the environment. And that slowly declines and is a shift after about 60 days. And the idea is that they're. Is a rapid a period where they need to avoid attacking to support the female. But after that period isn't that they will typically be left that scenario and they'll be back to their standard state and switching. So what we get out of this experiment, is it really true? Is it real physiological change induced by that process of mating? And so we can see a similar pattern to what's going on here in wild female mice. But that doesn't happen in laboratory female mice, intriguingly, that sort of they've been bred to be a bit more docile and nicer to pups. That's what ends up being used in lab experiments. They're not as aggressive. So what causes that? So this is a great grass in Davidson, could be in the wild watching this and describing it and say, Wow, look at this. Amazing behaviour is a remarkable behaviour we can see in the animal kingdom. But you're not in nature show. You're in a neurobiology lecture. Why? Why do we get this? Well, what these scientists have done their research is this their time dependent? So in that process, synaptic changes to the sign ups as there have a changing in those male mice brains and rest transcriptional. So that's within the DNA you're having different readout of the genes is a change in gene expression and triggered by that mating process. So that mating process causes the release of different chemical cues that change that. And some of them are released by females during pregnancy, can drive that radical behaviour shift from killing to parenting. What they found is if you disrupt the verbal maze Logan in wild male virgin males, it will reduce that process. If I go back to that graph here, you took a mouse, take a group of mice and you disrupt their vulnerable needs. Logan They normally would attack, but the ones we don't have the capacity to detect those cues drop down. They start behaving as if they mated. They're no longer detecting the signals that cause them to attack. So that formalised logic is not just useful for going, oh, detecting your friends or your children, but detecting strangers for the mice. Why they attacking these pups? Is it trying to preserve their own particular gene pool of their own genes? If they can make the mouse, the male mice can maybe all the other female mice and it can have its offspring grow up. The best thing it can do is kill off the other competitors children. And again, there are lots of other mammals that do this. Lions or the famous example, it will kill a new line and just the bride takes it will kill the the cubs from the other lions. That happens to be in that pride. But you also see these going to patterns and berry burying beetles. So not just vertebrates and lots of birds. The changes in the female are more varied. So you have the well known features to do with oestrogen. Progesterone and prolactin are all three hormones that circulate through the body in females during pregnancy that change that maternal behaviour in males. There's also testosterone very familiar culturally with the idea of testosterone is linked to aggression. It regulates a lot of that is very high in the males and it can be reduced in fathers to produce less aggressive behaviour. But of course this varies. I always remember all these things vary between people. That graph shows you this. There are some, you know, some animals, some of these males after after mating, they're still attacking and killing and there are some before they mated that are not doing that. So just be aware. There's a lot of variation out there too. Now, I've written on this slide down memory you do not need for the exam to memorise the layout and the interconnections of all these nuclei. That's not what this is about. This is a this is a figure four from that really great review. What you need to take away for this second module is that there are two of these circuits. One circuit is involved in parental care and one circuit. This involves an aggression, so particularly violence towards other strangers and pups. And the important point here is that these two circuits are linked. You can see the accessory olfactory bulb. That's the pathway you heard in the slide three where I talked about the vulnerable days and going to this area. And it projects into the aggressive aggression circuit. And if you damage that that circuit, it won't get turned on as much. They're not going to show as much aggression. The here we have another circuit involved in parental care that can get switched on. And instead they have they have these two interacting circuits. So they will, if one is on the axons from these brain areas, will terminate. And a lot of these other is to shut them down. They'll be negative impact and vice versa because you can't be both aggressive parenting at the same time. That's just because you just can't do the two different behaviours. So you notice to you there are a lot of other brain areas we come across. We've got here various areas in the amygdala that are important for Christmas, think parental care and we've got the prefrontal. Text. It's receiving and sending information back in. And you have again to remind you have a lecture at the end of the course and what the prefrontal cortex is doing by Professor Paul Burgess. So we come back to that. You can hold on to this for the moment that the prefrontal cortex, it shows a modulatory control in a lot of behaviour. One of them is the the exertion of parental control. So these two circuits exist and they they're antagonistic. What do we know about them? Is that the aversive circuit? First of all, that that red one is dominant. It's the one in charge most of the time in female virgin rats and male virgin rats. We talked about in lab female rats, mice and rats. The same sort of idea here that can can be quite docile. But generally that aggressive circuit is long. It will it will lead them to survive more. The aggression will save them as a as a rodent post-partum. So after mating and the desensitisation that occurs with in females, there's this hormonal neuro modulatory experience dependent factors that activate that, that other affiliated circuit that also acts to silence that that that the circuit, the one that does the aggression and avoidance. So just to highlight here it does this this is the avoidance and aggression. I've mainly talked about fighting, doing things, but this circuit is also important for just ignoring, running right, just not caring all these behaviours that again, are part of survival for for mice and rats. As part of us walks the toasting we talked about. That is really key in the initiation of that material behaviour. So we talked about if you inject these goals with oxidation, they won't thorns, but they also won't treat their children. They won't do the licking and grooming that we heard about under the stress lecture. So it really is important for oxytocin to be to be to be engaged for that. There's an area we're going to talk about next week in the detail of the ventral segmental area. We spend a lot of time talking about that and the molecules don't the mean and that's involved. It's a state just at this point, this brain areas involved in initiating this process and maintaining the behavioural maternal behaviour. So oxytocin is not shown here, but bonds on the receptors in the ventral segmental area and the ventral segmental areas you hear next week is the brain area that motivates and drives animals to do things again and again. So in this case, the drive here is that the animal is the, the, the females to really spend time investing with a the pups and be the partners. So that brain area is key for drives initiating and maintaining that behaviour. We'll come on to how it does that for all sorts of things. So people, the things we get involved in are habits, bad habits, Ferals that driven by this all depends on the situation. But beyond that there's also adrenaline and serotonin. Serotonin. These circuits are involved in maternal behaviours. Just to highlight in this picture here you have these various brain structures showing up here and these are the areas, the rough and the locus to release these two brain areas that release that involved in in regulating our attention, our arousal and our focus. These here appear to be part of that circuit that's involved in paternal care. And so these are also involved in that in that process. So just to end on, I'll talk through one experiment that just gives a really good two experiments. This one and the next one is a wrap up today's lecture. This is a figure from a news and Views article. So Rodriguez are writing up, trying to explain this experiment. But another group had done so. What they did in this experiment to understand the maternal paternal process for male and female is what's happening in their brains. And in this case they're looking at the medial pre optic area of the hypothalamus. So earlier on in this lecture, I showed you the hypothalamus and I explained this like a number of different nuclei within there. One of these is called the medial pre optic area. It's got nothing to do with optics. It just happens to be next to the optic nerve. So the medial pre optic area and after the animals have mated, you tend to get activation of neurones in this brain area. They're not as active before, but they become activated after mating this small bit of brain area. And that's what's indicated here by the red neurones. These are neurones that are sending out the highly active, the sending out transmission, That transmission of those neurones of the medial pre optic area they're arguing is driving that parental behaviour. And the reason they're finding these are critical is that they can go in and they can deplete those neurones just in that one tiny area. You barely see it in the brain. They've gone in and selectively damage those neurones by clever chemical techniques. But after that process you get no parental behaviour from the female or the male remaining looking at female mice and experienced male mice and experienced female mice. Like I mentioned, it's the virgin males where you don't get this pattern right. These all show this, this. If you damage this, you don't get this parental type behaviours. So that's the grooming, the retrieving pups and taking care of them. Okay, that's, that's one experiment to show it's important. That's one way of doing it. But ask these these four groups, virgin females, experienced males who mated and experienced females and mated experience here is little about mating on the right hand side. Now, this is for me. This is why this paper was published in science. That first experiments. Okay. It damaged the brain. They can't do it. That's fine. Very long. Here is the kicker. Experiment around. From a scientific perspective, it was virgin male mice. First of all, you can see they're not active. These cells that are active in these all three other categories of mice in the virgin males, they've not mated. They're just like young, aggressive male mice who have not made it yet. And what do they show? Aggression. They attack. They tap upset. They get rid of them. Here's what they did. They went in and artificially using optogenetics, find those cells that could be activated, activating and activate those cells by shining the laser light onto the genetically tagged cells. So what you can see is before and after that light is this this is a couple of milliseconds. You've got a normal functioning male. You shine a light. 2 milliseconds later, a whole of the cells are turned on. What does it do? So rather than running across the cage and attacking and possibly trying to bite and destroy pop, it runs over, it picks it up, puts it back in the nest. So by just turning on a light, affecting a small number of cells, more than the number of neurones in this illustration here, but still you're talking about tiny cell nucleus in a small part of the brain. You switched one animal from showing this aggressive behaviour to a whole different approach. And it's really worth what's quite amazing about this is it's not like you've improved motor function or its ability to perceive light in some way. You've changed the radical feature of its entire behaviour in a millisecond. You've with this experiment, they've turned to mice that would attack and kill. It's one artificially thinks it's mated effectively. It's not thinking about this, but you've artificially activated a parental circuit in an animal that's never undergone any change. So this shows the power of the kind of optogenetics approach of exploring behaviour. So you got to ask, why did you use the light? How do you see through this activity? Yes. So optogenetics, as you heard in your lecture on the methods lecture, remember that the methods, the techniques you learned about what you could do is genetically tagged those cells in these particular rats. I think it is for mice in mice more likely get mice. They've gone and bred these animals with specific proteins that are sensitive to the wavelengths of light. When those wavelengths of light are show shot, shine down on those cells through a cut in the top of the brain of the rat, the mouse, they can artificially turn them on and they can do it within a couple of milliseconds. And so you can have a rat, a mouse running across the cage to attack and switch the behaviour in an instant from attack to being attacking an aggressive and killing to being attacked in milliseconds through one circuit interruption. So what does that what makes that amazing is that it's quite a complex behaviour you're looking at. Now this is a experiment where they were looking at another particular nucleus. The this particular part, the AVP haven't read that, but it's in this paper. So this is a hypothalamic nucleus which differs between Virgin. So a difference between males and females in virgins. But what they showed is that this particular is not just the nucleus involved with the particular subcategories of neurones that can start to get to that. You can regulate in the parents, the females. So males after mating don't show a change here, but the female mice can show a change in the number of active neurones in this particular nucleus after mating. So not only do you get these patterns of expression, but you can get them in particular male or female situations as well. But it's worth going to watch these movies to see the patterns of behaviour. So in this link, if you don't follow it through, you will see the use of optogenetics where you get mice, which is not really fighting with each other, and then they'll turn on these these neurones artificially and they can do it in males as well. So they can turn on the mouse. Yes. This is almost certain case. Oh, yes. But this is all about. So if they might be right, they might not get children from mice. It's extremely likely their breeding capacity is way higher. That's one of the reasons they've ended up being laboratory animals that are really higher breeding. They require fast turnover. They can have children very quickly, but very likely they would, but it's not dependent. So these activation patterns are not dependent on the sex success. Well, I think you would need to activate pregnancy, though, so if they don't end up being pregnant. But I think the idea of the meeting alone will activate patterns of change in the male, because they can't detect that necessarily. But to pick up on that point, when the female does become pregnant, her pheromones secretions, what she's secreting, will change. And that provides a cue because humans, we don't really dissect these things but might do dogs do. They can detect other animals can smell these things. We can't in the same way those they go and watch these movies because you can see them turning off again with the optogenetics and the midpoints to attack, making an animal docile and parent and pick up a tree pups. It's it's really one of the most dramatic pieces of evidence I've seen. And again, one of the reasons it's published in the most prestigious journal in the world in Nature. So to wrap up today, there's a nice section in the physiology of behaviour that calls in or brisket on social bonding. That review is really clear and really nice to read into and Young. I talked about the voles again, extremely easy to read article and then there's some other reading. If you're really interested you could look into This is not essential, but we can go into that and we'll see you next week. The consciousness and motivation, why we do the things we do. |